08.02.2013 Views

Bernal S D_2010.pdf - University of Plymouth

Bernal S D_2010.pdf - University of Plymouth

Bernal S D_2010.pdf - University of Plymouth

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

2.1. OBJECT RECOGNJTION<br />

properties arise from retinal ganglion cells and LGN cells, which also exhibit some selectivity<br />

to contrast, velocity, colour and spatial frequency, V1 neurons with similar tuning properties<br />

lend to group together, leading t(i Ihe classical columnar organization <strong>of</strong> ocular dominance and<br />

orientation preference in cortex (Hubel and Wiesel 1965).<br />

Hubel and Wiesel (1965) were the first to propose the hierarchical organization <strong>of</strong> receptive<br />

tields, such thai VI simple cells are built from converging LGN cells aligned in space to pro­<br />

duce the elongated on-<strong>of</strong>f subregions observed. Additionally, the model provided the first clas­<br />

sification <strong>of</strong> VI ceils, dividing them into simple and complex. Cells fell into the simple category<br />

if their receptive fields could be separated into on and <strong>of</strong>f subregions, which could be linearly<br />

summaled to predict the cell's response to different artificial stimuli. The re,st <strong>of</strong> the cells, which<br />

did not have separate subregions, were categorized by exclusion as complex cells. However, the<br />

majority <strong>of</strong> VI cells fall into die complex category. As will be described further down, there are<br />

also numerous variants within the simple and complex categories.<br />

Several extensions improved the initial Hubel and Wiesel receptive held model <strong>of</strong> VI neurons.<br />

l-irstly, the linear filler was expanded to include a temporal dimension. Spatiolemporal receptive<br />

fields not only lake into account the spatial pr<strong>of</strong>ile, but also the temporal course <strong>of</strong> the response,<br />

and have proved to be crucial in understanding direction selectivity. This first filtering stage<br />

was .shown to be well approximated by 2-dimensional Gabor filters (Jones and Palmer 1987).<br />

Secondly, a nonlinear stage was added, which described how the linear filter outputs were trans­<br />

formed into an instantaneous firing rate via a nonlinear Poisson process. The two-stage model<br />

was therefore called the linear-nonlinear (l.N) model and provided a much better prediction <strong>of</strong><br />

neuron responses than strictly linear lilters, specially for retina and thalamic cells (Carandini<br />

elal.2(M)5).<br />

Nonetheless, the model still had significant limitations (Ringach 2004). It was unable to account<br />

for the dependence on contrast <strong>of</strong> several response properties, such as saturation and summation<br />

size. For example, the greater the contrast <strong>of</strong> the stimulus, the smaller the degree <strong>of</strong> spatial<br />

summation, and thus the receptive field size. Furthermore, the LN model could not explain<br />

surround suppression, such as stimuli at an orthogonal orientation inhibiting the cells' response.<br />

10

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!