30.06.2013 Views

Gram - SEAS

Gram - SEAS

Gram - SEAS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

94 4 Pragmatic factors<br />

4.5 Pragmatic enrichment versus "bleach ing"<br />

From very early times researchers on issues related to grammaticalization<br />

have observed that it involves loss of semantic content. Th is has been described<br />

by the metaphor of "fading" or "bleaching" (Gabelentz spoke of "verbleichen"<br />

'to grow pale,' Meillet of "affaiblissement" 'weakening'). More recently, Hl!l e_<br />

and Reh characterized grammaticalization as: "an evolution whereby linguistic<br />

units lose in semantic complex ity, pragmatic significance, syntactic freedom, and<br />

phonetic substance" (Heine and Reh 1984: 15). Readers will have noted that in<br />

this chapter we have, however, spoken of pragmatic enrichment, strengthening,<br />

and so forth. Th is is because we have been discussing the beginnings of grammaticalization,<br />

that is, the motivations that permit the process to begin, rather than its<br />

outcomes. There is no doubt that, over time, meanings tend to become weakened<br />

during the process of grammaticalization. Nevertheless, all the evidence for early<br />

stages is that initially there is a redistribution or shift, not a loss, of meaning.<br />

For example, with reference to the development of future go, Sweetser says:<br />

"we lose the sense of physical motion (together with all its likely background<br />

inferences). We gain, however, a new meaning of future prediction or intention -<br />

together with its likely background inferences" (Sweetser 1988: 392). In speaking<br />

of the subjectification of be going to, Langacker draws attention to the loss of<br />

objective locational reference points that movement entails, and suggests that this<br />

loss is replaced by realignment to the speaker's temporal perspective (1990: 23).<br />

In other words, one meaning is demoted, another promoted.<br />

As grammaticalized forms become increasingly syntacticized or morphologized<br />

they unquestionably cease over time to carry significant semantic or pragmatic<br />

meaning. This can most clearly be seen when former lexical items become empty<br />

syntactic clements, as in the·case of do, or when formerly separate morphemes become<br />

bound and serve primarily as "morphological detritus" after repeated fusion<br />

(sec Chapter 6). An excellent example is provided by the development of French<br />

ra 'that,' a form which is the worn-down relic of several stages of expressive<br />

reinforcement:<br />

(30) hoc 'that' > (ecce) hoc 'behold that' > eccehoc > ,,0 > ce > ce(lli) 'thut<br />

there' > celli > .. a (LUdtke 1980: 212)<br />

The individual meanings of hoc, ecce, and La have been lost, as has the form's<br />

distal demonstrative fu nction (M. Harris 1978: Chapter 4).<br />

Two general working principles arise out of our understanding of the processes<br />

of inferencing in grammaticalization. One is that the meanings will always be<br />

derivable from the original lexical meaning by either metaphorical or conceptual<br />

metonymic inferencing. Therefore meaning changes in grammaticalization are not

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!