08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

#P185 POSTER SESSION IV:<br />

CHEMICAL SIGNALING AND BEHAVIOR;<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR/PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

CHEMOSENSATION AND METABOLISM;<br />

VOMERONSASAL AND CHEMICAL<br />

COMMUNICATION<br />

#P186 POSTER SESSION IV:<br />

CHEMICAL SIGNALING AND BEHAVIOR;<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR/PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

CHEMOSENSATION AND METABOLISM;<br />

VOMERONSASAL AND CHEMICAL<br />

COMMUNICATION<br />

Social Olfactory Cues and Stress<br />

Pamela Dalton, Cristina Jaen, Tamika Wilson, Christopher Maute<br />

Monell Chemical Senses Center Philadelphia, PA, USA<br />

Olfactory cues have the potential to precipitate emotional<br />

responses and thereby alter mood, judgments and behavior.<br />

In prior work, we demonstrated that exposure to a novel odor<br />

while undergoing a laboratory stressor caused individuals<br />

to re-experience stress (increased heart rate & self-reported<br />

stress) when re-exposed to that odor three days later. We<br />

wished to evaluate the potential <strong>for</strong> odor to alter stress levels<br />

following a standard laboratory stressor. One class of olfactory<br />

stimuli which has shown promise in eliciting robust effects on<br />

mood and emotion are social odors. Lundström et al. (2008)<br />

demonstrated that smelling a stranger’s body odor activated<br />

a marked response in the amygdala of subjects, despite a low<br />

conscious recognition of the odor or its source. In this study,<br />

we evaluated the changes in autonomic stress levels following<br />

the Trier Social Stress Test among individuals in 3 groups who<br />

were exposed to either the body odor of their sibling, the body<br />

odor of a stranger or a non-social (fragrance) odor. Axillary<br />

odors were collected from non-twin, whole, biological siblings<br />

who were then recalled to participate in the main study in which<br />

one of the 3 odors was administered following the stress task.<br />

Results showed a significant decrease in post-recovery heart<br />

rate only among the group smelling the sibling odor, whereas<br />

skin conductance was significantly reduced <strong>for</strong> both the sibling<br />

odor and the fragrance. Following a stressor, exposure to the<br />

stranger body odor maintained arousal levels longer suggesting<br />

that both familiar and stranger body odors may be potent cues<br />

<strong>for</strong> emotional responses. Acknowledgements: Supported by the<br />

U.S. Army Research Office grant # W911NF-11-1-0087, entitled<br />

“Learning & Olfaction: Understanding and Enhancing a Critical<br />

Communication Channel”.<br />

Can a chemosensory threat be masked?<br />

Amy R Gordon 1,2 , Kathrin Ohla 2,3 , Mats J Olsson 1 ,<br />

Johan N Lundstrom 1,2,4<br />

1<br />

Karolinska Institutet Stockholm, Sweden, 2 Monell Chemical Senses<br />

Center Philadelphia, PA, USA, 3 German Institute of Human<br />

Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbrücke, Germany, 4 University of<br />

Pennsylvania Philadelphia, PA, USA<br />

The well-established angry advantage effect has been extended<br />

in recent crossmodal visual-olfactory studies using schematic<br />

human faces and human body odors. In a threat-detection task,<br />

humans detect an angry (threatening) schematic face in an<br />

array of neutral distracter faces more quickly than a friendly<br />

(non-threatening) face, hence the label ‘Angry advantage’.<br />

We have previously shown that the body odor of unknown<br />

individuals (Strangers) – an established threatening olfactory<br />

stimulus – speeds a subject’s detection of threatening faces, but<br />

not non-threatening faces, relative to exposure to the subject’s<br />

own body odor (Self). Using event-related potential (ERPs),<br />

we have more recently demonstrated that the presence of<br />

a Strangers’ body odor causes a non-threatening face to be<br />

processed as a threatening stimulus. In the present ERP study,<br />

we sought to determine whether the chemosignal mediating the<br />

a<strong>for</strong>ementioned ERP effect could be masked by a common odor<br />

(Mask). Angry and neutral schematic faces were presented to<br />

subjects in the presence of Strangers’ body odor + Mask, ‘Self’<br />

body odor + Mask, or Mask only control, which were delivered<br />

intra-nasally by a computer-controlled olfactometer. Preliminary<br />

analyses suggest that even in the presence of an odor mask,<br />

exposure to the body odor of a stranger, relative to the odorless<br />

control and ‘Self’ body odor, results in significant differences<br />

in the late (cognitive) components of visual processing. This<br />

suggests that body odor can modulate the cognitive evaluation<br />

of visual stimuli even in the presence of a perceptual odor mask.<br />

The effects of masked body odor and common odor exposure<br />

on visual processing will be presented and discussed within the<br />

framework of the adaptive advantages conveyed by heightened<br />

sensitivity to threat-related stimuli. Acknowledgements: This<br />

work was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and<br />

other Communication Disorders – NIDCD (R03DC009869)<br />

awarded to JNL<br />

POSTER PRESENTATIONS<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s).<br />

100

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!