08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

study was to develop a paradigm <strong>for</strong> testing declarative memory<br />

processes with regard to odors. There<strong>for</strong>e, we implemented<br />

three memory tasks in E-prime 2.0 presentation software. With<br />

help of an odor-place associative memory task we investigated<br />

memory of odor-place combinations. During an odor item<br />

recognition task the subjects were asked if they had experienced<br />

several odors during the previous task. As a control task <strong>for</strong> the<br />

first task a picture-place associative memory task was utilized.<br />

Each of those tasks contained two phases – an encoding phase (1<br />

block) and a retrieval phase (4 blocks) – and were applied to 17<br />

healthy subjects. The results suggest that the subjects were able<br />

to learn during the encoding as well as during the retrieval phase.<br />

We found higher odor-place associative, odor recognition, and<br />

picture-place associative memory per<strong>for</strong>mance scores at the end<br />

compared to the beginning of the retrieval phase. We thus claim<br />

that our paradigm renders useful during the investigation of<br />

olfactory memory processes and the influence of external factors<br />

on those processes in humans. We here present a fully automated<br />

paradigm, which can be utilized during future functional imaging<br />

studies with the goal to shed light onto the neural network of<br />

human olfactory memory processes. Acknowledgements: This<br />

research was funded by a startup grant from the Medical Faculty<br />

of RWTH Aachen University.<br />

groups of at least 10 Ss on over a dozen such materials.<br />

We used the CPL protocols that have given stable results and<br />

required no normalization. The outcome fell in line with the<br />

data <strong>for</strong> the hundreds of materials, with neither more nor less<br />

variance. It adds evidence that the LSER has universal relevance<br />

to predict potency. Since the solvation-relevant parameters<br />

used <strong>for</strong> prediction exist <strong>for</strong> thousands of materials, the LSER<br />

may af<strong>for</strong>d more accessibility and accuracy than values in<br />

compilations. Acknowledgements: Supported by International<br />

Flavors and Fragrances.<br />

#P130 POSTER SESSION III:<br />

TRIGEMINAL; HUMAN OLFACTORY<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS; TASTE PERIPHERY<br />

Categorical Dimensions of Human Odor Descriptor Space<br />

Revealed by Non-Negative Matrix Factorization<br />

Jason B Castro 1 , Arvind Ramanathan 2 , Chakra S Chennubhotla 3<br />

1<br />

Bates College, Department of Psychology, Program in Neuroscience<br />

Lewiston, ME, USA, 2 Oak Ridge National Laboratory Oak Ridge, TN,<br />

USA, 3 University of Pittsburgh, Department of Computational and<br />

Systems Biology Pittsburgh, PA, USA<br />

#P129 POSTER SESSION III:<br />

TRIGEMINAL; HUMAN OLFACTORY<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS; TASTE PERIPHERY<br />

Fragrance Materials Extend the Range of a Model <strong>for</strong><br />

Odor Potency<br />

William S Cain 1 , Roland Schmidt 1 , Jorge E Cometto-Muñiz 1 ,<br />

Sang Park 1 , Craig B Warren 1 , Michael H Abraham 2 , Matthias Tabert 3<br />

1<br />

Chemosensory Perception Lab (CPL) La Jolla, CA, USA, 2 University<br />

College London / Chemistry London, United Kingdom, 3 International<br />

Flavors & Fragrances Union Beach, NJ, USA<br />

Olfactory research offers few trustworthy odor thresholds.<br />

Bad <strong>for</strong> the field, the situation sets nonexperts in need of the<br />

in<strong>for</strong>mation adrift. A rule of thumb says to choose the lowest<br />

threshold of a set because studies with the best stimulus control<br />

and some measure of concentration normally yield the lowest<br />

values. Nagata et al. over years produced the largest set of<br />

thresholds gathered with coherent methodology and analytical<br />

verification. They lie below most others. Abraham et al. used a<br />

well-established linear solvation energy relationship (LSER) to<br />

describe the set. To strengthen the position, they incorporated<br />

sets from the CPL and Hellman. Data obtained via olfactometer<br />

in recent years from the CPL needed no normalization with the<br />

Nagata set, whereas those from Hellman and earlier squeezebottle<br />

data from the CPL did. The LSER accommodated these<br />

via indicator variables (factors) to bring the data into line and<br />

described potency <strong>for</strong> 353 odorants. Although the equation left<br />

about 25% of variance unaccounted <strong>for</strong>, it offers the strongest<br />

statement yet <strong>for</strong> prediction. Until now, no sets included<br />

fragrance materials, such as patchouli oil or vanillin, known <strong>for</strong><br />

potency. Without them, one could argue that the equation might<br />

describe local rather than universal rules. We accordingly ran<br />

Recent studies using Principal Components Analysis (PCA)<br />

support low-dimensional models of odor space, in which one or<br />

two dimensions – with hedonic valence featuring prominently –<br />

explain most odor variability. Here we use non-negative matrix<br />

factorization (NMF) – a nonlinear optimization method - to<br />

discover an alternative, reduced-dimensional representation<br />

of the Dravnieks odor database(144 odors x 146 descriptors).<br />

We first divided the dataset into training and testing halves,<br />

and found that RMSD testing error attained a minimum <strong>for</strong><br />

subspace choice of 25, motivating this as an upper bound <strong>for</strong><br />

odor perceptual space dimensionality. More parsimonious<br />

representations were found by comparing reconstruction errors<br />

(fraction of unexplained variance) of NMF with reconstruction<br />

errors of PCA on scrambled data (PCAsd). For subspace<br />

sizes > 10, NMF error was indistinguishable from PCAsd<br />

error, indicating no gain in retaining more than 10 perceptual<br />

dimensions. As is typical of NMF basis sets, the 10 odor<br />

dimensions we obtain are sparse (only a small subset of the<br />

146 descriptors apply), and categorical (represent a positive<br />

valued quality). Moreover, these 10 dimensions were nearorthogonal,<br />

with a mean angle of 73 degrees between all pairs<br />

of basis vectors. Investigating the distribution of odors in this<br />

10-dimensional space, we find marked clustering, with each<br />

odor clearly defined by its membership in a single dimension, to<br />

the exclusion of others. Members of each cluster have notable<br />

structural homology, which we quantified as correlations among<br />

physiochemical descriptors of odors in each cluster. In sum, we<br />

describe a representation of odor perceptual space consisting of<br />

10 discretely occupied dimensions that apply categorically. We<br />

propose that this may help elucidate the natural axes of olfaction.<br />

Acknowledgements: NIH GM086238 (CSC)<br />

POSTER PRESENTATIONS<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s).<br />

78

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!