08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

#P212 POSTER SESSION IV:<br />

CHEMICAL SIGNALING AND BEHAVIOR;<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR/PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

CHEMOSENSATION AND METABOLISM;<br />

VOMERONSASAL AND CHEMICAL<br />

COMMUNICATION<br />

#P213 POSTER SESSION IV:<br />

CHEMICAL SIGNALING AND BEHAVIOR;<br />

ANIMAL BEHAVIOR/PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

CHEMOSENSATION AND METABOLISM;<br />

VOMERONSASAL AND CHEMICAL<br />

COMMUNICATION<br />

Licking Microstructure Reveals Rapid Attenuation of<br />

Neophobia<br />

Kevin J Monk, Benjamin D Rubin, Jennifer Keene, Donald B Katz<br />

Brandeis University Waltham, MA, USA<br />

Neophobia—the initial hesitation that many animals show to a<br />

novel food—is typically measured by comparing consumption<br />

in the first and second sessions of access to the taste; lower<br />

consumption in session 1 denotes neophobia, and higher 2ndsession<br />

consumption denotes attenuation of neophobia (AN).<br />

AN is thought to represent a bona fide example of learning—<br />

neural plasticity induced by an association between the taste and<br />

a safe outcome on session 1 changes the response to the tastant<br />

during session 2. Such long-term plasticity processes require time<br />

to complete, and thus AN should only stabilize 90 min or more<br />

following the first exposure to the tastant—a prediction that has<br />

been borne out in behavioral data. It remains possible, however,<br />

that a more rapidly developing AN might escape detection<br />

in time-averaged accounts of behavior such as consumption.<br />

With this in mind, we per<strong>for</strong>med a comparison of AN in two<br />

contexts, a two-bottle test and a brief access test (which allowed<br />

a real-time analysis of licking microstructure). At the level of<br />

overall consumption, data from the two tasks were in good<br />

accord—both revealed AN to 28mM saccharin but not to 2.8mM<br />

saccharin. Additionally, however, the brief access task revealed<br />

an initial hesitation to consume the higher concentration<br />

saccharin solution; this seemingly neophobia-related hesitation<br />

not only decreased between sessions 1 and 2, but also decreased<br />

linearly across the twenty minutes of session ; that is, AN began<br />

within minutes of the rats’ first exposure to the taste. These<br />

data validate the brief-access task as an paradigm with which to<br />

measure AN, and also reveal aspects of AN—perhaps related<br />

to short-term plasticity—that appear within minutes of the first<br />

taste. Acknowledgements: National Institutes of Health, World<br />

of Work Fellowship<br />

Channelrhodopsin Mice use Temporal In<strong>for</strong>mation Encoded<br />

in the Olfactory Bulb <strong>for</strong> Odor Sensation.<br />

Michelle R. Rebello 1,2 , Thomas S. McTavish 2 , David C. Willhite 1,2 ,<br />

Gordon M. Shepherd 2 , Justus V. Verhagen 1,2<br />

1<br />

John B. Pierce Laboratory New Haven, CT, USA, 2 Yale School of<br />

Medicine, Dept. of Neurobiology New Haven, CT, USA<br />

Odor in<strong>for</strong>mation is represented by spatio-temporal maps in the<br />

olfactory bulb (OB). Spatial maps reflect the converging axons<br />

of olfactory receptor neurons activated by odors, onto their<br />

respective glomeruli in the OB. The origins of temporal patterns<br />

of glomerular activation are less well understood, but odorant<br />

receptor affinity as well as odorant sorption kinetics across<br />

the olfactory epithelium could underlie temporal parameters<br />

such as onset latency and rise time. Consistent differences in<br />

response dynamics across glomeruli have been found <strong>for</strong> odorevoked<br />

responses in the OB. Further, we have shown, using<br />

optical imaging that retronasal and orthonasal bulbar reponses<br />

differ in response amplitude as well as temporal dynamics. It<br />

is there<strong>for</strong>e evident that rich temporal in<strong>for</strong>mation is available<br />

in the bulbar response. However it is not known whether these<br />

temporally dynamic responses are behaviorally relevant. Using<br />

transgenic mice expressing ChR2 under the Thy-1 promoter in<br />

the mitral cells and a digital micromirror device to project snifftriggered<br />

light patterns onto the dorsal OB we are able to exert<br />

tight spatio-temporal control over OB activity patterns. We find<br />

that mice trained on a go/no-go task are able to discriminate<br />

patterns that are spatially identical but differ temporally. By<br />

varying the relative delay among the same regions activated by<br />

light patterns we are able to determine the threshold of temporal<br />

discrimination. We find that Thy-1 ChR2 but not wild-type<br />

mice can make temporal discriminations of less than 30ms.<br />

Our optogenetic study confirms that awake, behaving mice<br />

can use temporal in<strong>for</strong>mation encoded in the bulbar response.<br />

This suggests that temporal coding can contribute to retronasal<br />

and orthonasal odor sensation. Acknowledgements: This work<br />

is supported by NIH/NIDCD Grants R01DC009994 and<br />

R01DC011286 and NIH Institutional Training Grant T15-<br />

LM007056 from the National Library of Medicine.<br />

POSTER PRESENTATIONS<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s).<br />

112

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!