08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

#P240 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

The role of gene expression in the human TAS2R38<br />

genotype-phenotype relationship<br />

Sarah V. Lipchock 1 , Andrew I. Spielman 2 , Julie A. Mennella 1 ,<br />

Danielle R. Reed 1<br />

1<br />

Monell Chemical Senses Center Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2 College of<br />

Dentistry, New York University New York, NY, USA<br />

Bitter taste is important <strong>for</strong> sensing and avoiding toxins, but the<br />

aversion to bitter can lead to low consumption of vegetables,<br />

an important source of phytochemicals. Single-nucleotide<br />

polymorphisms in the genes that code <strong>for</strong> the 25 human bitter<br />

receptors (TAS2Rs) result in different individual responses to<br />

taste stimuli. One example of this phenomenon is the TAS2R38<br />

gene, which encodes a receptor that recognizes compounds<br />

including 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) and goitrin, found<br />

in broccoli. Cell-based assays and psychophysical threshold<br />

measurements confirm that receptor with the amino acid<br />

sequence PAV responds to PROP, while the AVI <strong>for</strong>m does not.<br />

Heterozygous (PAV/AVI) individuals display a broad range of<br />

abilities to taste PROP. To examine the role of allele-specific<br />

gene expression in the TAS2R38 genotype-phenotype relationship<br />

we recruited healthy, non-smoking adults with the PAV/AVI<br />

diplotype (N=18). Psychophysical ratings <strong>for</strong> several bitter<br />

compounds and vegetable juices were related to levels of TAS2R<br />

gene expression from human fungi<strong>for</strong>m taste papillae. Increased<br />

levels of the PAV <strong>for</strong>m of TAS2R38 were positively correlated<br />

with the subject’s intensity ratings <strong>for</strong> PROP and broccoli juice.<br />

Expression of TAS2R43 was associated with ratings <strong>for</strong> caffeine,<br />

urea, denatonium benzoate and quinine. This relationship<br />

resulted from copy number variation of TAS2R43 and suggests<br />

that TAS2R43 acts as a proxy <strong>for</strong> bitterness sensitivity from the<br />

cluster of receptors on chromosome 12. Our data provide a link<br />

between genotype and phenotype in the taste system. Our work<br />

lays a foundation <strong>for</strong> future studies about regulatory mechanisms<br />

and implications of taste receptor expression as it relates to diet<br />

and vegetable consumption in humans. Acknowledgements:<br />

Supported by F32DC011975 (SVL), P30DC011735 (DRR) and<br />

R01DC011287 (JAM).<br />

#P241 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

Evidence that cooling affects the sweetness of sugars via 2<br />

separate mechanisms.<br />

Danielle J. Nachtigal 1 , Barry G. Green 1,2<br />

1<br />

The John B. Pierce Laboratory New Haven, CT, USA, 2 Department of<br />

Surgery, Yale University School of Medicine New Haven, CT, USA<br />

Previous experiments in our laboratory demonstrated that<br />

temperature can affect the sweetness of sucrose, glucose, and<br />

fructose solutions by modulating taste adaptation: Cooling from<br />

37°C to 21°C significantly increased the rate and amount of selfadaptation<br />

when the tongue tip was dipped into the solutions <strong>for</strong><br />

several seconds. However, cooling to 21°C did not reduce initial<br />

sweet taste intensity, and thus did not directly affect sensitivity<br />

to the sugars. In the present study we investigated whether<br />

cooling to colder temperatures would further accelerate sweet<br />

taste adaptation and/or begin to reduce sweet taste sensitivity.<br />

Subjects rated the sweetness of 0.42 M sucrose solutions at<br />

temperatures of 41°, 37°, 30°, 21°, 10° or 5°C after dipping<br />

the tongue tip <strong>for</strong> 0, 3, or 10 sec into either 0.42 M sucrose or<br />

pure dH 2<br />

O of the same temperature. Sweetness ratings were<br />

made prior to retracting the tongue back into the mouth. There<br />

were two main findings: (1) sweet taste adaptation increased<br />

significantly at 21°C (replicating our previous results), and<br />

(2) pre-exposure to pure dH 2<br />

O at 5° or 10°C significantly<br />

reduced sweet taste intensity independent of taste adaptation.<br />

These findings indicate that cooling interferes with the<br />

perception of sucrose sweetness in two different ways: mild<br />

cooling increases the rate of adaptation whereas more extreme<br />

cooling reduces the initial sensitivity to sucrose. Experiments<br />

are planned that will test the hypothesis that these two effects<br />

occur at different stages of the transduction cascade, with the<br />

<strong>for</strong>mer affecting the binding of sucrose to the transmembrane<br />

region of the T1R2-T1R3 receptor, and the latter modulating a<br />

temperature-sensitive intracellular mechanism that is common to<br />

G-protein coupled receptors (e.g., TRPM5). Acknowledgements:<br />

NIH grant RO1 DC005002<br />

#P242 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

The Chemosensory Component in the 2012 National Health<br />

and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES): Test-Retest<br />

Analysis and Comparison with Laboratory-Based Measures<br />

Shristi Rawal 1 , Howard J Hoffman 2 , Kathy Bainbridge 2 ,<br />

Valerie B Duffy 1<br />

1<br />

University of Connecticut/Allied Health <strong>Sciences</strong> Storrs, CT, USA,<br />

2<br />

National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders/<br />

Division of Scientific Programs Bethesda, MD, USA<br />

The NHANES now includes a standardized chemosensory<br />

protocol with brief assessments of taste intensity and odor<br />

identification conducted in a mobile exam center. After<br />

orientation to the general Labeled Magnitude Scale and scaling<br />

intensity of LED-generated lights, participants report intensities<br />

of 1 M NaCl and 1 mM quinine hydrochloride applied to the<br />

tongue tip and these plus 0.32 M NaCl with the whole mouth.<br />

Olfactory dysfunction is assessed with two 4-item, scratchand-sniff<br />

tests (Pocket Tests (PT), Sensonics, Inc) with food<br />

(strawberry, chocolate, onion, grape) and common (leather,<br />

soap, smoke, natural gas) odors. Here we examined test-retest<br />

reliabilities of the chemosensory protocol, compared the PT<br />

with an olfactometer identification task, and added scaling<br />

to all odor tests. Fifty adults (mean age=27, range 18-62 yrs),<br />

primarily Caucasian and Asian, college-educated and reportedly<br />

healthy, were tested in a laboratory twice within two weeks.<br />

POSTER PRESENTATIONS<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s).<br />

123

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!