08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

#P229 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

Are individuals with elevated food liking scores (‘foodies’)<br />

hypergeusic?<br />

Nadia K Byrnes, Alissa L Allen, Emma L Feeney, Rachel J Primrose,<br />

John E Hayes<br />

Department of Food Science University Park, PA, USA<br />

The public believes that foodies are supertasters (and vice versa),<br />

consistent with reports that chefs and wine experts report greater<br />

bitterness from propylthiouracil (PROP). However, the two<br />

reports that test this hypothesis conflict. Minski et al. reported<br />

‘high food interest’ individuals (‘foodies’) rated quinine, sodium<br />

chloride and PROP as more intense than those with average or<br />

low food interest in a laboratory study (n=100), while Pickering<br />

et al. failed to find any difference in the bitterness of PROP<br />

impregnated discs sent via mail (n>900). These studies also<br />

differ in how high food affect individuals were identified: via a<br />

ratio of affective ratings <strong>for</strong> all foods to pleasant non-food item<br />

versus a difference score of mean liking <strong>for</strong> all foods minus mean<br />

liking <strong>for</strong> all non-foods. Here, we explore this question in 246<br />

subjects who completed a generalized hedonic survey (i.e. food<br />

& nonfood items) and whole mouth ratings <strong>for</strong> sucrose, quinine,<br />

and PROP. We characterized subjects as high affect using both<br />

approaches. Regardless of the categorization method, sucrose,<br />

quinine, and PROP intensity ratings did not differ by group in<br />

ANOVA. When groups were predicted in logistic regression<br />

(‘do higher taste ratings predict being a foodie?’) there was no<br />

evidence supporting this hypothesis. Adding the personality trait<br />

Sensation Seeking as a covariate did not alter these conclusions.<br />

We did find evidence of lower Sensation Seeking scores<br />

among foodies (as defined via the difference score), but further<br />

inspection suggests this was due to a small increase in the mean<br />

liking of pleasant non-food items when mean food liking was<br />

flat. These data fail to support the hypotheses that a) hypergeusic<br />

individuals show higher food related affect or that b) higher food<br />

affect predicts heightened taste response. Acknowledgements:<br />

funds from the Pennsylvania State University and NIH grant<br />

DC0010904.<br />

#P230 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

Effects of Food Neophobia on Salivary ph, Cortisol and<br />

Adrenal Level<br />

August Capiola, Bryan Raudenbush, Amanda Schultz<br />

Wheeling Jesuit University Wheeling, WV, USA<br />

Food neophobics (individuals reluctant to try novel foods)<br />

and food neophilics (individuals with an overt willingness<br />

to try novel foods) differ in several physiological aspects.<br />

Phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) tasting ability, a genetic<br />

predisposition, differs among the two groups with more food<br />

neophobics possessing this inherited trait. Food neophobics<br />

salivate less when presented with novel foods and have higher<br />

physiological stress responses to novel foods (increased pulse,<br />

GSR, and respirations). The present study assessed salivary<br />

pH, adrenal level and cortisol level in food neophobics, food<br />

neophilics and an average group, to determine whether such<br />

salivary flow and physiological stress reactions could partially be<br />

explained by such variables. Salivary mouth swab samples were<br />

obtained from 117 participants, who also completed the Food<br />

Neophobia Scale (FNS) to assess level of food neophobia. A<br />

significant MANCOVA result was found, F=2.47, p=.03. Further<br />

analysis revealed food neophobics had significantly higher levels<br />

of salivary cortisol compared to food neophilics and the average<br />

group, F(2,102)=7.53, p=.001. The finding that higher levels<br />

of the stress hormone cortisol are present in food neophobic’s<br />

saliva supports past research indicating a greater physiological<br />

stress reaction to novel food stimuli in these individuals. Future<br />

research should assess whether exposure to novel foods can<br />

decrease the level of salivary cortisol in food neophobics, as a<br />

way of promoting a more varied and healthful diet.<br />

#P231 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

HUMAN TASTE PSYCHOPHYSICS;<br />

OLFACTION RECEPTORS; TASTE DEVELOPMENT<br />

The NIH Toolbox Brief Gustation Assessment Protocol<br />

Susan E. Coldwell 1 , Valerie B. Duffy 2 , Linda Bartoshuk 3 ,<br />

James W. Griffith 4 , Howard J. Hoffman 5<br />

1<br />

University of Washington School of Dentistry Seattle, WA, USA,<br />

2<br />

University of Connecticut College of Agriculture and Natural<br />

Resources Storrs, CT, USA, 3 University of Florida College of Dentistry<br />

Gainesville, FL, USA, 4 Northwestern University Feinberg School of<br />

Medicine Evanston, IL, USA, 5 National Institutes of Health, National<br />

Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders Bethesda,<br />

MD, USA<br />

NIH Toolbox developed standardized, brief assessments <strong>for</strong><br />

sensory, motor, cognitive and emotional function that are<br />

designed <strong>for</strong> use in longitudinal studies, epidemiological<br />

research, and clinical trials. The sensory battery consists of<br />

brief assessments of gustation, olfaction, vision, audition, pain,<br />

and vestibular balance; all six can be administered within 30<br />

minutes, including 6 minutes <strong>for</strong> the assessment of gustation.<br />

The Gustation Assessment begins with instructions in use of the<br />

general Labeled Magnitude Scale by rating of remembered lights<br />

(dimly lit restaurant, well-lit room, brightest light ever seen).<br />

Four taste trials are then delivered: 1 mM Quinine HCl applied<br />

to the anterior tongue, 1 M NaCl applied to the anterior tongue,<br />

1 mM Quinine HCl whole mouth (sip and spit), and 1 M NaCl<br />

whole mouth. Rinsing with water is done between trials. As<br />

part of the NIH Toolbox national norming study, the Gustation<br />

Assessment was given to 1843 English-speakers and 240 Spanishspeakers.<br />

These included 494 subjects aged 12 to 15 years and<br />

509 aged 15 to 19 years. For 172 subjects, the battery was given<br />

twice to establish test-retest reliability. Preliminary intraclass<br />

correlations (ICC) indicate that the test is reliable <strong>for</strong> whole<br />

mouth ratings (ICC = 0.54 <strong>for</strong> Quinine and 0.57 <strong>for</strong> NaCl) and<br />

reliable <strong>for</strong> NaCl on the anterior tongue (ICC = 0.42). Quinine<br />

ratings <strong>for</strong> the anterior tongue were less reliable (ICC = 0.29),<br />

POSTER PRESENTATIONS<br />

<strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s).<br />

119

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!