22.03.2013 Views

et al.

et al.

et al.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

BOOKS ET AL.<br />

760<br />

senting hundreds of millions of pages such as<br />

the full text of Wikipedia. Y<strong>et</strong> except for these<br />

restricted speci<strong>al</strong> domains, AI remains a long<br />

way from its human variant.<br />

Kurzweil introduces the reader to one of<br />

AI’s signature mathematic<strong>al</strong> techniques that<br />

he himself used to build speech recognition<br />

systems: hierarchic<strong>al</strong>, hidden Markov models<br />

(HHMMs). These are trainable <strong>al</strong>gorithms<br />

well matched to de<strong>al</strong> with the many levels of<br />

human language, from phonemes to words<br />

to sentences and beyond. For certain types<br />

of simple problems, HHMMs can be trained<br />

fairly automatic<strong>al</strong>ly and are easy to implement<br />

in “units” that are connected by ascending<br />

and descending links.<br />

It is here that Kurzweil leaves the solid<br />

ground of his expertise to wade into the muddier<br />

waters of biology and psychology. For<br />

he now categoric<strong>al</strong>ly asserts that HHMMs<br />

provide a powerful and univers<strong>al</strong> model of<br />

neocortic<strong>al</strong> computation, with 300 million<br />

pattern recognizers distributed across the<br />

cortic<strong>al</strong> she<strong>et</strong>.<br />

Kurzweil introspects into his own mind’s<br />

capabilities and the nature of perception and<br />

memory, without bothering to refer to the<br />

massive literature on these topics. One of the<br />

most basic lessons of psychology is that we<br />

have little idea of what goes on in our minds,<br />

as evolution has not given us access to most<br />

parts of the brain (explaining why so much<br />

philosophy of mind has been barren when <strong>al</strong>l<br />

it could rely on was introspecting philoso-<br />

BROWSINGS<br />

phers). He then moves on to the brain, describing<br />

its anatomy in terms of the “new” brain,<br />

higher-order regions of the neocortex, and the<br />

“old” brain (everything else). According to<br />

Kurzweil, the new brain is clever, learns fl exibly,<br />

and controls the primitive impulses of<br />

the old brain relating to food, sex, and aggression.<br />

His understanding of neuroanatomy is<br />

about as sophisticated as U.S. Secr<strong>et</strong>ary of<br />

Defense Don<strong>al</strong>d Rumsfeld’s understanding<br />

of internation<strong>al</strong> politics when he articulated<br />

his belief of a division of Europe into an Old<br />

and a New one during the run-up to the second<br />

Gulf War in 2003.<br />

Kurzweil’s knowledge of neuroscience is<br />

simply inadequate to the task at hand. From the<br />

tens of thousands of studies published annu<strong>al</strong>ly,<br />

he selectively cites a handful of papers<br />

that buttress his points, without giving any<br />

context. He mistakes the striatum for cortex<br />

and apic<strong>al</strong> dendrites for axons, belies the cognitive<br />

contributions of the bas<strong>al</strong> ganglia, and<br />

denies higher ment<strong>al</strong> abilities to insects, ceph<strong>al</strong>opods,<br />

and birds that don’t have a neocortex.<br />

Y<strong>et</strong> he has the unerring belief of the proph<strong>et</strong><br />

(or the fool): “I maintain that the model I have<br />

presented is the only possible model that satisfi<br />

es <strong>al</strong>l of the constraints that the research and<br />

our thought experiments have established.”<br />

Scientists trying to simulate the mindbrain<br />

f<strong>al</strong>l <strong>al</strong>ong a continuum, ranging from<br />

extreme biologic<strong>al</strong> chauvinism (the need to<br />

consider every ionic channel, action potenti<strong>al</strong>,<br />

and neuron to fully do justice to the<br />

Mathematic<strong>al</strong> Excursions to the World’s Great Buildings. Alexander J. Hahn. Princ<strong>et</strong>on University<br />

Press, Princ<strong>et</strong>on, NJ. 2012. 344 pp. $49.50, £34.95. ISBN 9780691145204.<br />

While touring examples of western architecture<br />

from the great pyramids of Giza<br />

to Frank Gehry’s Bilbao Guggenheim<br />

Museum, mathematician Hahn intertwines<br />

two historic<strong>al</strong> narratives: The architectur<strong>al</strong><br />

focuses on aspects of appearance (shapes,<br />

symm<strong>et</strong>ry, and proportion) and mechanics<br />

(loads, compressions, tensions, and<br />

thrusts). The mathematic<strong>al</strong> progresses from<br />

Euclidean geom<strong>et</strong>ry and trigonom<strong>et</strong>ry to<br />

basic c<strong>al</strong>culus. His discussions reve<strong>al</strong> how<br />

mathematics provides insights into the<br />

design and construction of the buildings<br />

and how the buildings incarnate the math.<br />

[Carefully composed using circles and squares, Leon Battista Alberti’s 15th-century facade (above)<br />

for the mediev<strong>al</strong> church Santa Maria Novella in Florence was a source of inspiration to a number of<br />

Renaissance architects.] As Hahn notes, his two strands are linked topic<strong>al</strong>ly rather than chronologic<strong>al</strong>ly,<br />

because the actu<strong>al</strong> builders gener<strong>al</strong>ly lacked the mathematics that clarifi es understanding of their<br />

constructions. Readers wishing to shore up their own understanding of that mathematics can work<br />

through the chapter-end problem s<strong>et</strong>s and discussions.<br />

15 FEBRUARY 2013 VOL 339 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org<br />

Published by AAAS<br />

baroque complexity of the brain’s circuits) to<br />

the austerity of a purely <strong>al</strong>gorithmic approach<br />

of replicating the mind in software (the mind<br />

is not w<strong>et</strong>, after <strong>al</strong>l). Mathematicians and<br />

engineers natur<strong>al</strong>ly belong to the later camp,<br />

convinced that one <strong>al</strong>gorithm rules them<br />

<strong>al</strong>l. Previous favorites include logic<strong>al</strong> c<strong>al</strong>culus,<br />

neur<strong>al</strong> n<strong>et</strong>works, cellular automata, selfreferenti<strong>al</strong><br />

programs, and, yes, hidden<br />

Markov models [as proposed a few years earlier<br />

by fellow entrepreneur and inventor of the<br />

P<strong>al</strong>mPilot Jeff Hawkins ( 1)].<br />

Kurzweil correctly points out that the<br />

pace at which biologists accumulate data<br />

has increased dramatic<strong>al</strong>ly over the years<br />

(<strong>al</strong>though I only wish that his claim that “the<br />

spati<strong>al</strong> resolution of brain scanning … [is]<br />

doubling every year” were true). From this<br />

he infers that a compl<strong>et</strong>e understanding of the<br />

brain and the mind can’t be far away.<br />

Paradoxic<strong>al</strong>ly, the endless data fields<br />

make it ever more diffi cult to distinguish the<br />

sign<strong>al</strong> from the noise. Indeed, the torrent of<br />

data beg<strong>et</strong>s the illusion of progress. While<br />

data about the brain accumulate exponenti<strong>al</strong>ly,<br />

our understanding increases sublinearly.<br />

Basic questions about cortic<strong>al</strong> circuitry<br />

posed by future Nobel laureates David Hubel<br />

and Torsten Wiesel in a celebrated publication<br />

in 1962 ( 2) remain unanswered 50 years<br />

later. Function<strong>al</strong> human brain imaging has<br />

y<strong>et</strong> to affect standard medic<strong>al</strong> practice (the<br />

upcoming fi fth edition of the Diagnostic and<br />

Statistic<strong>al</strong> Manu<strong>al</strong> of Ment<strong>al</strong> Disorders does<br />

not even mention any function<strong>al</strong> magn<strong>et</strong>ic<br />

resonance imaging diagnostic criteria). And<br />

even the lowly roundworm Caenorhabditis<br />

elegans, a creature no bigger than the l<strong>et</strong>ter<br />

l and with exactly 302 nerve cells, is for now<br />

beyond the ability of computation<strong>al</strong> neuroscience<br />

to comprehend. Kurzweil’s claim that<br />

we will soon fi gure out how the 100 billion<br />

neurons of the human brain function on the<br />

basis of designed HHMMs is compl<strong>et</strong>e bosh.<br />

One thing is certain. Biology knows nothing<br />

of simplicity. Brains are not assembled<br />

out of billons of identic<strong>al</strong> LEGO blocks but<br />

out of hundreds of distinct nerve cell types.<br />

Each cell type has its own idiosyncratic morphology,<br />

sign<strong>al</strong>ing, and active genes. And<br />

they are interconnected with elaborate wiring<br />

rules that we only discern darkly. To paraphrase<br />

Winston Churchill, neuroscience is<br />

(perhaps) at the end of the beginning of the<br />

quest to understand our brain and mind.<br />

References<br />

1. J. Hawkins, S Blakeslee, On Intelligence (Times Books,<br />

New York, 2005).<br />

2. D. H. Hubel, T. N. Wiesel, J. Physiol. 160, 106 (1962).<br />

10.1126/science.1233813<br />

CREDIT: JEBULON/WIKIMEDIA COMMONS<br />

on February 14, 2013<br />

www.sciencemag.org<br />

Downloaded from

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!