22.11.2012 Views

Fieldwork and Linguistic Analysis in Indigenous ... - ScholarSpace

Fieldwork and Linguistic Analysis in Indigenous ... - ScholarSpace

Fieldwork and Linguistic Analysis in Indigenous ... - ScholarSpace

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Noun Class <strong>and</strong> Number <strong>in</strong> Kiowa-Tanoan 83<br />

revealed to a degree. The follow<strong>in</strong>g section will discuss what has been found concern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the semantic basis for the categories. However, s<strong>in</strong>ce there are still open questions about<br />

the distribution of nouns across noun classes, the historical semantics of the <strong>in</strong>verse will<br />

rema<strong>in</strong> unresolved for the present.<br />

It must be borne <strong>in</strong> m<strong>in</strong>d that the <strong>in</strong>verse system did not necessarily have a transparent<br />

semantic basis <strong>in</strong> Proto-Kiowa-Tanoan. 27 Proto-languages are of course still languages,<br />

unattested though they may be, <strong>and</strong> are just as likely to show arbitrary properties as synchronic<br />

languages. Arbitrary as grammars may end up be<strong>in</strong>g, however, they usually do not<br />

start out that way. Constructions <strong>and</strong> patterns tend to have an orig<strong>in</strong>al semantic or formal<br />

motivation which is then obscured over time by grammaticalization processes. The questions<br />

driv<strong>in</strong>g this paper are targeted towards these orig<strong>in</strong>al motivations, even though they<br />

may no longer have been apparent by the time of the immediate ancestor of the modern<br />

Kiowa-Tanoan languages. That such a typologically unusual <strong>in</strong>verse number system is not<br />

found <strong>in</strong> any other language family—<strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g Uto-Aztecan, which some have posited<br />

has a connection to Kiowa-Tanoan (Whorf <strong>and</strong> Trager 1937, Davis 1989)—suggests that<br />

the system <strong>and</strong> the motivations for the system arose after an ancestor of the Kiowa-Tanoan<br />

languages had split from other relations. Only further research will be able to determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

the possible character of Proto-Kiowa-Tanoan <strong>and</strong> whether the <strong>in</strong>verse system had any<br />

transparent semantic basis <strong>in</strong> that language or if it had been lost by this time.<br />

3.4. GENDER AND NUMBER IN HISTORICAL KIOWA-TANOAN NOUN CLASSES. Why<br />

nouns fall <strong>in</strong>to the classes that they do <strong>in</strong> each of the languages is not entirely clear. If it<br />

were based solely upon f<strong>in</strong>e number differentiations, one would expect more variation <strong>in</strong><br />

the number mark<strong>in</strong>g of a given noun, depend<strong>in</strong>g upon the construal necessary <strong>in</strong> the given<br />

context, as discussed at the end of section 2.4. Additionally, there would not be the semantic<br />

patterns that are quite prevalent <strong>in</strong> the languages. The most pervasive of these patterns is<br />

the restriction of animate nouns to class I <strong>and</strong>, moreover, the restriction of class I to animate<br />

nouns, with very few exceptions. Even <strong>in</strong> Kiowa, where more <strong>in</strong>animates are classed <strong>in</strong> I<br />

than <strong>in</strong> the other languages, they still are relatively few <strong>in</strong> number. This provides strong<br />

evidence that the languages are at the very least mak<strong>in</strong>g an animate-<strong>in</strong>animate dist<strong>in</strong>ction<br />

<strong>and</strong> that this dist<strong>in</strong>ction is reflected <strong>in</strong> the number mark<strong>in</strong>g pattern. 28 The question then<br />

arises whether there is a further semantic basis between <strong>in</strong>animate nouns <strong>in</strong> their respective<br />

classes, i.e., II, III, <strong>and</strong> IV.<br />

Class IV nouns, which appear <strong>in</strong> all languages but Kiowa to consist entirely of <strong>in</strong>animate<br />

non-count <strong>and</strong> abstract nouns, are obviously dist<strong>in</strong>guished for their unique number<br />

properties, namely that no number dist<strong>in</strong>ction tends to be made. This is a class determ<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

solely by number semantics <strong>and</strong> does not seem to have any <strong>in</strong>teraction with any gender<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ctions, outside of the fact that most or all non-count <strong>and</strong> abstract nouns are <strong>in</strong>animate.<br />

27 Thanks to an anonymous reviewer for rem<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g the author to explicate this po<strong>in</strong>t.<br />

28 The dist<strong>in</strong>ction between animates <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>animates is well attested elsewhere <strong>in</strong> the languages. See<br />

especially Allen, Gard<strong>in</strong>er, <strong>and</strong> Frantz (1984), Rosen (1990), <strong>and</strong> Sadock (1985) for Southern Tiwa;<br />

Yumitani (1998) for Towa.<br />

fieldwork <strong>and</strong> l<strong>in</strong>guistic analysis <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>digenous languages of the americas

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!