01.10.2014 Views

An Introduction to Critical Thinking and Creativity - always yours

An Introduction to Critical Thinking and Creativity - always yours

An Introduction to Critical Thinking and Creativity - always yours

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

CONTEXT BIAS 187<br />

• Recent experiences have a greater impact than earlier ones. This is known<br />

as the recency effect. This is perhaps one reason why many lawyers present<br />

their most important witnesses near the end of the trial.<br />

20.2 CONTEXT BIAS<br />

A context bias is a bias in our judgment triggered by irrelevant features of the situation<br />

in which the judgment in made. These features might have <strong>to</strong> do with the<br />

way a problem is presented, or are features of the environment that have very little<br />

<strong>to</strong> do with the problem we are trying <strong>to</strong> solve.<br />

20.2.1 Presentation effects<br />

What is interesting about the third warmup question is that the 50 million figure<br />

in the question has a big influence on the answers that people come up with. Typical<br />

guesses might be between 30 <strong>and</strong> 100 million. Now ask your friends the same<br />

question, but replace the 50 million with the more accurate estimate of 5 million.<br />

The average answer is likely <strong>to</strong> be much smaller. However, surely the question<br />

does not imply in any way that the number mentioned is close <strong>to</strong> the correct answer.<br />

This is an illustration of anchoring, where we arrive at our judgments by<br />

making minor adjustments <strong>to</strong> some arbitrary reference point given <strong>to</strong> us (the anchor).<br />

We can therefore manipulate judgments by changing the reference point.<br />

In a vivid demonstration of the anchoring effect, MIT professor Dan Ariely<br />

asked his students <strong>to</strong> write down the last two digits of their Social Security number<br />

(a form of identity in the United States). The students then bid on items such<br />

as wine <strong>and</strong> chocolates in an auction. It turns out that those who wrote down a<br />

higher number are more willing <strong>to</strong> offer a higher bid, sometimes by almost 100<br />

percent. Clearly their perception of what counts as a fair price had been unconsciously<br />

biased by some completely irrelevant information.<br />

In sales <strong>and</strong> marketing, the anchoring effect can be used <strong>to</strong> influence consumers.<br />

Some researchers asked real estate agents <strong>to</strong> inspect a house <strong>and</strong> estimate<br />

its value. Earlier, some agents were given a higher list price, some a lower one. The<br />

list price is the anchor that affected the agents' estimates, even though the agents<br />

were supposed <strong>to</strong> be experts. On average, those who saw the higher list price gave<br />

a higher estimate. When asked, they also denied having taken the list price in<strong>to</strong><br />

account. Instead, they would cite features of the property <strong>to</strong> justify their estimates<br />

(Northcraft <strong>and</strong> Neale, 1987).<br />

The anchoring effect also offers lessons for business negotiations. The traditional<br />

wisdom is that you wait for the other party <strong>to</strong> make a move first, because<br />

the offer tells you what he or she might be thinking. But research suggests that a<br />

party making the first offer can sometimes gain an advantage by skewing the final<br />

outcome through anchoring (Galinsky, 2004).<br />

Here are more examples of how context affects our decisions without our conscious<br />

awareness:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!