13.07.2015 Views

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Proceedings</strong>, FOETIK <strong>2009</strong>, Dept. of Linguistics, Stockholm Universityther, the open class words often refer to concreteobjects in the physical world and mighttherefore be learned earlier (Gentner & Boroditsky,2001). Nor are children expected to useclose class words until they have reached certaingrammatical maturity (Håkansson, 1998).The youngest subjects in the current studywere 1.2-years old and some of the forms completedearly on – with vocabularies < 50 words– did not contain any close class words. Shortlythereafter – for vocabularies > 50 words, all thechildren can be assumed to have reachedgrammatical maturity. The current study doesthus not reveal the exact time point for startingto use close class words. Nevertheless, the agegroup of the current study ranged between 1.2-years to 3.6-years and likely captured the timepoint for onset of word spurt. The onset ofword spurt has been documented to take placesometime between the end of the first and theend of the third year of life (Bates et al., 1994).Therefore, the proportional increase of closeclass words being almost twice as large (17%)for vocabulary size of 300 to 600 words, ascompared to vocabulary size from 100 to 300words (10%) is not surprising.One reason for expecting close class wordsto later enter the children’s vocabularies is thatchildren might have more difficult to understandthe abstract meaning of close class words.But closer inspection of the results shows thatchildren start to use close class words althoughthe size of their vocabularies is still relativelysmall. For example, one of the subjects showedat one occasion to have one close class wordand five open class words. But a question to beasked next is how close class words are used inchild language. That is, has the child understoodthe abstract functions of the words used? Itis reasonable that children use close class wordsto express other functions than the originalfunction of the word in question. For example,the word upp (eng. up) might not be the understoodas the abstract content of the prepositionup, but instead used to refer to the action lyftmig upp (eng. lift med up). Using the particle ofa verb and omitting the verb referring to the actionis typical in child language (Håkansson,1998). Thus, the close class words are oftenphonotactically less complex (compare upp tolyfta) and therefore likely more available for thechild. But the use of the word per se does notindicate that the child has understood thegrammatical role of the close class words in thelanguage. The close class words used by the 14-to 43-month-old children in the current studywere Pronouns, Time expressions, prepositions/wordsfor spatial locations, word forAmount and articles, Auxiliary verbs, Connectorsand question words. It may thus be speculatedthat the children in the current study havestarted to perceive and explore the grammaticalstatus of the close class words.AcknowledgementsResearch supported by The Bank of SwedenTercentenary Foundation, and European Commission.We thank Ingrid Broomé, AndreaDahlman, Liz Hultby, Ingrid Rådholm, andAmanda Thorell for data analysis within theirB-level term paper in Logopedics.ReferencesBates, e., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L.,Dale, P., Reilly, J., Hartung, J. (1994) Developmentaland stylistic variation in the compositionof early vocabulary. Journal ofChild Language 21, 85-123.Eriksson, M. and Berglund, E. (1995) Instruments,scoring manual and percentile levelsof the Swedish Early Communicative DevelopmentInventory, SECDI, FoUnämnden.Högskolan i Gävle.Gentner, D. and Boroditsky, L. (2001) Individuation,relativity, and early word learning.In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. (eds)Language acquisition and conceptual development,215-256. Cambridge UniversityPress, UK.Håkansson, G. (1998). Språkinlärning hos barn.Studentlitteratur.Klintfors, E., Lacerda, F., and Sundberg, U.(2007) Estimates of Infants’ vocabularycomposition and the role of adultinstructionsfor early word-learning.<strong>Proceedings</strong> of <strong>Fonetik</strong> 2007, TMH-QPSR(Stockholm, Sweden) 50, 69-72.Kuhl, K. P., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A,Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V.,Ryskina V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg,U. and Lacerda, F. (1997) Cross-languageanalysis of phonetic units in language addressedto infants. Science, 277, 684-686.Strömqvist, S. (1997) Om tidig morfologiskutveckling. In Söderberg, R. (ed) Från jollertill läsning och skrivning, 61-80. Gleerups.Strömqvist, S. (2003) Barns tidigaspråkutveckling. In Bjar, L. and Liberg, S.(eds) Barn utvecklar sitt språk, 57-77.Studentlitteratur.Weijer, J. van de (1998) Language input forword discovery. Ph.D thesis. Max PlanckSeries in Psycholinguistics 9.129

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!