13.07.2015 Views

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

Proceedings Fonetik 2009 - Institutionen för lingvistik

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Proceedings</strong>, FONETIK <strong>2009</strong>, Dept. of Linguistics, Stockholm Universityfor each condition (cf. the example in the Introduction).While the frame context was presentedto the subjects exclusively in writtenform, the context question was only presentedauditively. For that, the context questions werepre-recorded by a 30-year-old male nativespeaker of Swedish.Recording procedure and subjectsThe data collection was performed using acomputer program, which both presented thecontexts and test sentences to the subjects andorganised the recording. First, for each trial,only the frame context was displayed on thescreen in written form. The subjects had to readthe context silently and to try to imagine thesituation described in the context. When ready,they clicked on a button to continue with thetrial. Then, the pre-recorded context questionwas played to them via headphones, and simultaneously,the test sentence appeared on thescreen. The subject’s task was to answer thequestion using the test sentence in a normalconversational style. The subjects were allowedto repeat each trial until they were satisfied.Besides the material for this study, the recordingsession included a number of further testcases not reported on in this paper. Five repetitionsof each condition were recorded, and thewhole list of items was randomised. One recordingsession took about 15 minutes perspeaker. Nine speakers of Standard Swedishwere recorded (5 female) in an experimentalstudio at the Humanities Laboratory at LundUniversity. Thus, a corpus of 270 utterancesrelevant to this study (6 conditions, 5 repetitionsper speaker, 9 speakers) was collected.Data analysisA first step in data analysis is reported in Ambrazaitis(in press). There, the goal was to providean overview of the most salient characteristicsof the F0 patterns produced in the differentconditions. To this end, F0 contours were timeand register normalised, and mean contourswere calculated in order to illustrate the generalcharacteristics of the dominant patterns found inthe different conditions (cf. examples in Figure1). The F0 patterns were classified according tothe F0 movement found in connection with thestressed syllable of the target word, as either‘falling’ or ‘rising’.In order to obtain duration measurements, inthe present study, the recorded utterances weresegmented into 10 quasi-syllables using spectrogramsand wave form diagrams. Theboundaries between the segments were set asillustrated by the following broad phonetictranscriptions: [ʋa], [ˈland], [əɹ], [fœ], [ˈlɛŋː], [əɹ],[tɪl], [nɔ], [ˈvɛmb], [əɹ]. In the case of [ˈland] and[ˈvɛmb], the final boundary was set at the time ofthe plosive burst, if present, or at the onset of thepost-stress vowel.It has been shown for Swedish that focallengthening in assertions is non-linear, in thatthe stressed syllable is lengthened more than theunstressed syllables (Heldner and Strangert,2001). Therefore, durational patterns were analysedon two levels, first, taking into accountentire word durations, second, concentrating onstressed syllables only. In both cases, theanalyses focussed on the three content wordsand hence disregarded the word “till”.For each word, two repeated-measuresANOVAs were calculated, one with word durationas the dependant variable, the other forstressed syllable duration. In each of the sixANOVAs, there were three factors: SPEECH ACT(with two levels: assertion, confirmation), FO-CUS (POSITION) (three levels: focus on initial,medial, final word), and finally REPETITION(five repetitions, i.e. five levels).All data were included in these sixANOVAs, irrespective of possible mispronunciations,or the intonation patterns produced (cf.the two strategies for confirmations, Figure 1),in order to obtain a general picture of the effectsof focus and speech act on duration. However,the major issue is whether focus in confirmationsmay be signalled by a falling F0 pattern.Therefore, in a second step, durational patternswere looked at with respect to the classificationof F0 patterns made in Ambrazaitis (in press).ResultsFigure 2 displays mean durations of the threetest words for the six conditions (three focuspositions in two speech acts). The figure onlyshows word durations, since, on an approximatedescriptive level, the tendencies for stressedsyllable durations are similar; the differencesbetween durational patterns based on entirewords and stressed syllables only will, however,be accounted for in the inferential statistics.The figure shows that the final word (“november”)is generally produced relatively longeven when unfocussed, i.e. longer than medialor initial unfocussed words, reflecting thewell-known phenomenon of final lengthening.Moreover, the medial word (“<strong>för</strong>länger”) is74

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!