09.06.2013 Views

Crossroads: The Psychology of Immigration in the New Century

Crossroads: The Psychology of Immigration in the New Century

Crossroads: The Psychology of Immigration in the New Century

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Immigrant Populations <strong>in</strong><br />

Educational Contexts<br />

<strong>The</strong> size and diversity <strong>of</strong> today’s immigration flow is<br />

reflected <strong>in</strong> U.S. public schools. As <strong>of</strong> 2011, 23.7%<br />

<strong>of</strong> school-age children <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> United States were <strong>the</strong><br />

children <strong>of</strong> immigrants (MPI, 2011), <strong>the</strong> majority<br />

(77%) <strong>of</strong> whom were second-generation citizen children and<br />

<strong>the</strong> rest (23%) foreign-born (Ma<strong>the</strong>r, 2009). Approximately<br />

10.7% <strong>of</strong> all public school students <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> United States are<br />

classified as English language learners (ELLs) 6 (MPI, 2011).<br />

<strong>The</strong>re is tremendous diversity <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> socioeconomic, cultural,<br />

and l<strong>in</strong>guistic backgrounds <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>se children. For example,<br />

elementary and middle-school children <strong>in</strong> <strong>New</strong> York City<br />

public schools speak 167 languages and come from 192<br />

countries (Stiefel, Schwartz, & Conger, 2003).<br />

a resilience Perspective<br />

<strong>The</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong> high achievement among many firstgeneration<br />

immigrants is remarkable given <strong>the</strong> myriad<br />

challenges <strong>the</strong>y encounter, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g xenophobia,<br />

economic obstacles, language difficulties, family separations,<br />

underresourced neighborhoods and schools, and <strong>the</strong> struggle<br />

to get <strong>the</strong>ir bear<strong>in</strong>gs <strong>in</strong> a new educational system (V. W.<br />

Huynh & Fuligni, 2008; Pong & Hao, 2007; Portes & Zhou,<br />

6 Emerg<strong>in</strong>g bil<strong>in</strong>guals is a term preferred by some scholars (see O.<br />

García, 2009; O. García, Kleifgen, & Falchi, 2008; Reyes & Azuara,<br />

2008) who emphasize <strong>the</strong> bil<strong>in</strong>gual competencies <strong>of</strong> immigrantorig<strong>in</strong><br />

children and advocate for educational policies that develop<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir home language and cultural understand<strong>in</strong>gs. In this report,<br />

we use <strong>the</strong> term English language learner (ELLs), as it emphasizes<br />

<strong>the</strong> common experience <strong>of</strong> acquir<strong>in</strong>g English primarily (though<br />

not exclusively) <strong>in</strong> a school environment and is more <strong>in</strong>clusive <strong>of</strong><br />

multil<strong>in</strong>gual and multidialectal children (e.g., <strong>in</strong>digenous peoples<br />

from Oaxaca <strong>of</strong> Mexico speak dialects <strong>of</strong> Mixteca; for <strong>the</strong>m Spanish<br />

is already <strong>the</strong>ir second language. Such is also <strong>the</strong> case for <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />

from <strong>the</strong> Fujian prov<strong>in</strong>ce <strong>in</strong> Ch<strong>in</strong>a; Mandar<strong>in</strong> is <strong>the</strong>ir second<br />

language). <strong>The</strong> use <strong>of</strong> this term is <strong>in</strong> no way <strong>in</strong>tended to denigrate<br />

<strong>the</strong> value <strong>of</strong> home language and cultures or advocate for <strong>the</strong><br />

superiority <strong>of</strong> English monol<strong>in</strong>gualism; <strong>in</strong> fact, ELL replaces limited<br />

English pr<strong>of</strong>icient, a term used by <strong>the</strong> federal government, which did<br />

perpetuate a deficit perspective <strong>of</strong> this population. While <strong>the</strong> term<br />

English language learner refers to a fluid category <strong>of</strong> students who<br />

do not meet levels <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>iciency <strong>in</strong> school sett<strong>in</strong>gs (variously def<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

across contexts) and is problematic <strong>in</strong> this respect, it is <strong>the</strong> term<br />

currently most widely used by researchers, policymakers, and school<br />

districts.<br />

Immigrant Populations <strong>in</strong> Educational Contexts<br />

1993). Immigrant children demonstrate certa<strong>in</strong> advantages.<br />

<strong>The</strong>y enter U.S. schools with tremendous optimism (Kao<br />

& Tienda, 1995), high aspirations (Fuligni, 2001; Portes &<br />

Rumbaut, 2001), dedication to hard work, positive attitudes<br />

toward school (C. Suárez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco, 1995),<br />

and an ethic <strong>of</strong> family support for advanced learn<strong>in</strong>g (Li,<br />

2004). As mentioned <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>troduction, immigrant youth<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten educationally outperform <strong>the</strong>ir U.S.-born peers<br />

(Perreira, Harris, & Lee, 2006).<br />

First-generation immigrant students show a number<br />

<strong>of</strong> positive academic behaviors and attitudes that <strong>of</strong>ten<br />

lead to stronger than expected academic outcomes. For<br />

example, compared wth <strong>the</strong>ir U.S.-born peers, <strong>the</strong>y have<br />

better attendance rates (García Coll & Marks, 2011),<br />

demonstrate more positive attitudes toward <strong>the</strong>ir teachers<br />

(C. Suárez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco, 1995) and school<br />

(Fuligni, 1997), have higher attachment to school (García<br />

Coll & Marks, 2011; C. Suárez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco,<br />

1995), and earn higher grades (García Coll & Marks,<br />

2011; Hernandez, Denton, McCartney, & Blanchard, 2011;<br />

Portes & Rumbaut, 2006). Some age groups have higher<br />

scores on standardized tests than do <strong>the</strong>ir Americanborn<br />

peers (García Coll & Marks, 2011), particularly on<br />

standardized math tests (Kao & Tienda, 1995). At <strong>the</strong> same<br />

time, a decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> academic aspirations, engagement, and<br />

performance has been documented over time (C. Suárez-<br />

Orozco, Gaytán, Bang, et al., 2010) and across generations<br />

(Fuligni, 1997; Portes & Rumbaut, 2001; C. Suárez-Orozco<br />

& Suárez-Orozco, 1995).<br />

On some measures <strong>of</strong> achievement and for some groups, firstgeneration<br />

students do not perform as well as <strong>the</strong>ir U.S.-born<br />

peers. First, because <strong>the</strong> first generation must contend with<br />

language acquisition, <strong>the</strong>ir performance suffers on tests <strong>of</strong><br />

read<strong>in</strong>g and English (Kao & Tienda, 1995; Ruiz-de-Velasco,<br />

Fix, & Clewell, 2000; C. Suárez-Orozco et al., 2008; N. Tran<br />

& Birman, 2010) when compet<strong>in</strong>g with U.S.-born peers.<br />

Second, a particularly difficult situation emerges for <strong>the</strong> group<br />

<strong>of</strong> immigrant children with <strong>in</strong>terrupted or no prior education.<br />

53

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!