06.08.2013 Views

Untitled

Untitled

Untitled

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

B1.4 MJ from protein in men, did not differ significantly<br />

from the other meals, which had similar energy density and<br />

‘dietary fibre’ content (although they differed in their<br />

sensory attributes, such as taste and after taste). There was<br />

also no difference in intake among the 10 women who<br />

consumed B0.7 MJ of protein from the high-protein meal.<br />

This study did not covertly manipulate the meals to conceal<br />

taste. Nevertheless, all the above observations taken together<br />

raise questions about the predictive value of macronutrient-specific<br />

models of feeding behaviour, at least under<br />

laboratory conditions, which may not necessarily apply to<br />

free-living conditions, for reasons that will emerge later.<br />

Inevitably, attention is focused on the weight and energy<br />

density of foods, including ready-to-eat foods, which have<br />

recently flooded the market in developed countries.<br />

Before considering these issues further, it is worth briefly<br />

reflecting on the likely limitations of physiological processes<br />

controlling feeding behaviour that place disproportionate<br />

importance on a single nutrient (for example, carbohydrate).<br />

Such a system would regulate energy balance poorly because<br />

it would be unable to adapt adequately to situations<br />

associated with changes in the proportions of different<br />

macronutrients in the food supply. Therefore, it is not<br />

surprising that specific macronutrient models of feeding<br />

behaviour were not found to be robust in some circumstances,<br />

particularly those involving covert manipulation of<br />

diets with the same energy density but different macronutrient<br />

composition (see above). In addition, pharmacological<br />

inhibition of both fat and glucose oxidation in animals were<br />

found to elicit a massive increase in food intake, compared to<br />

inhibition of either fat or carbohydrate oxidative pathways<br />

(Friedman and Tordoff, 1986). This suggests that the<br />

regulatory system is responsive to oxidation of both fat<br />

and carbohydrate, and that oxidation of each substrate<br />

compensates for the other, when the availability of one of<br />

them is reduced. Therefore, eating behaviour does not<br />

unconditionally depend on the oxidation of one nutrient,<br />

and indeed it would be surprising if it did. Furthermore, the<br />

satiating effect of the Eskimo diet (fat and protein) (Mowat,<br />

1981) would argue against the operation of a simple<br />

carbohydrate oxidation or storage model of feeding behaviour<br />

to the exclusion of other macronutrients.<br />

Energy density, weight of food and carbohydrates<br />

Several reports suggest that under ad libitum feeding conditions<br />

(mainly under laboratory conditions), people tend to<br />

consume a fixed weight or volume of food (Duncan et al.,<br />

1983; Lissner et al., 1987; Tremblay et al., 1991; Tremblay,<br />

1992; Poppitt, 1995; Rolls and Bell, 1999; Bell and Rolls,<br />

2001). This implies that when foods or diets differ in energy<br />

density, energy intake will be affected. Indeed, it has been<br />

suggested that this is a major mechanism for determining<br />

energy intake at different stages of the life cycle (for example,<br />

compared to young adults, older adults reduce energy intake<br />

by largely decreasing the energy density of the food eaten). If<br />

Physiological aspects of energy metabolism<br />

M Elia and JH Cummings<br />

satiety was mainly determined by the weight of food eaten,<br />

then those who consume high-energy density foods would<br />

be expected to feel satiated only after increased energy<br />

intake, whereas those consuming low-energy density foods<br />

would feel satiated despite lower energy intake. This<br />

argument has been promoted by several workers (Poppitt,<br />

1995; Poppitt and Prentice, 1996; Prentice and Poppitt, 1996;<br />

Rolls and Bell, 1999). It is argued that in real-life energy<br />

dense foods tend to be more palatable and less satiating than<br />

foods with low-energy density. Typically, palatable snacks are<br />

energy dense, and those rich in both fat and available<br />

carbohydrates, which are rapidly digested and absorbed, are<br />

said to be particularly palatable (Drewnowski, 1998). Interestingly,<br />

available carbohydrate and fat also provide more<br />

NME per unit ME than does the diet as a whole or either<br />

protein or NSP or other carbohydrates fermented in the large<br />

bowel. Therefore, when using the ME system, high-fat, highavailable<br />

carbohydrate foods (or diets) are more fattening<br />

than isoenergetic high-protein, high-fibre foods. In addition,<br />

foods of low-energy density (often rich in NSP and water)<br />

were less palatable in these studies.<br />

An examination of 1032 foods revealed that the strongest<br />

predictors of energy density are water, which is negatively<br />

related to energy density, and fat (g per 100 g food), which is<br />

positively related to energy density (Stubbs et al., 2000). The<br />

carbohydrate and protein content of foods (g per 100 g food)<br />

are both positively related to energy density, but the<br />

association is much weaker than those obtained with fat or<br />

water (Figure 4). This weak relationship with carbohydrate is<br />

understandable because it is possible for foods containing a<br />

large proportion of energy from carbohydrate to have either<br />

a low-energy density (for example, vegetables) or highenergy<br />

density (some sweet snacks). Diet surveys do not<br />

generally report the energy density of the food eaten, and<br />

this precludes extensive analyses and interpretation of data<br />

Energy density (kJ/100g)<br />

4000<br />

3500<br />

3000<br />

2500<br />

2000<br />

1500<br />

1000<br />

500<br />

0<br />

0 25 50<br />

Weight (g/100g)<br />

75 100<br />

Fat (r 2 =75)<br />

Protein (r 2 =13.9)<br />

Carbohydrate (r 2 =1.9)<br />

Water (r 2 =66.7)<br />

Figure 4 Relationship between energy density (outcome variable)<br />

and % weight of dietary macronutrients and water of 1032 readyto-eat<br />

foods (Stubbs et al., 2000). The prediction equations<br />

are as follows: Energy density (ED) ¼ 462.6 þ (35.5 fat); ED ¼<br />

781.4 þ (12.2 protein); ED ¼ 654.5 þ (12.5 carbohydrate); and<br />

ED ¼ 2034–(21.2 water). The values in parentheses, derived from<br />

regression are r 2 100.<br />

S55<br />

European Journal of Clinical Nutrition

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!