27.12.2013 Views

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

esponses are easier (i) for problems involving small as compared to large operands (e.g., 2 x<br />

3 is easier than 8 x 7, e.g., Stazyk, Ashcraft, & Hamann, 1982), (ii) for problems involving 5<br />

as an operand (e.g., 8 x 5 is easier than 8 x 4; Siegler, 1988) and (iii) for tie problems (e.g., 7<br />

x 7, Lefevre et al., 1996). Additionally, Campbell & Graham (1985; see also Butterworth,<br />

Marchesini, & Girelli, 2003; Campell, 1994; 1997) found that <strong>the</strong> majority <strong>of</strong> multiplication<br />

errors are so-called operand errors in which <strong>the</strong> erroneous result represents <strong>the</strong> correct result<br />

<strong>of</strong> a neighbouring problem [i.e., in which one <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> operands is mistakenly decreased or<br />

increased by 1, e.g., 4 x 6 = 28 which would be correct for 4 x (6 + 1)]. Evidence for<br />

decomposed processing <strong>of</strong> tens and units in multiplication comes from two observations.<br />

First, Verguts and Fias (2005a) were able to show in a computational modelling approach that<br />

problem size, five, and tie effect can be accounted for by a model assuming an initially<br />

decomposed representation <strong>of</strong> tens and units <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> correct result. Second, Verguts and Fias<br />

(2005b; see also Domahs, Delazer, & Nuerk, 2006; Domahs et al., 2007) introduced <strong>the</strong><br />

concept <strong>of</strong> unit-decade consistency to account for performance specificities in multiplication<br />

as described above. In this context consistency basically reflects <strong>the</strong> extent to which two<br />

neighbouring problems share <strong>the</strong> same decade or unit digit [e.g., 4 x 8 = 32; consistent probe:<br />

36 = 4 x (8 + 1) vs. inconsistent probe: 28 = 4 x (8 – 1)]. <strong>The</strong> finding that in multiplication<br />

verification decade consistent, but never<strong>the</strong>less incorrect solutions probes, are harder to reject<br />

than decade inconsistent probes (Domahs et al., 2007, see Domahs et al., 2006; Verguts &<br />

Fias, 2005b for data from a production task again argues for a decomposed representation <strong>of</strong><br />

two-digit <strong>number</strong>s. In this vein, processing a decade digit identical to <strong>the</strong> decade digit <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

correct result might drive <strong>the</strong> decision in one direction even when this decision bias is not<br />

confirmed by <strong>the</strong> processing <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> unit digit, <strong>the</strong>reby suggesting separate processing <strong>of</strong> tens<br />

and units.<br />

19

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!