27.12.2013 Views

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

The influence of the place-value structure of the Arabic number ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sources <strong>of</strong> difficulty in carry addition problems<br />

On a <strong>the</strong>oretical/conceptual level difficulties arising with <strong>the</strong> requirement <strong>of</strong> at least<br />

one carry have been suggested to originate from different sources. On <strong>the</strong> one hand, Kalaman<br />

and LeFevre (2007) argued that <strong>the</strong> carry effect represents an increased demand on working<br />

memory resources for e.g., keeping track <strong>of</strong> intermediate results (see also Imbo et al., 2007;<br />

Kazui, Kitagaki, & Mori, 2000; Zago et al., 2001). On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r hand, Nuerk, Graf and<br />

Willmes (2006) associated <strong>the</strong> increased difficulty <strong>of</strong> carry addition problems with a higher<br />

workload for <strong>the</strong> correct classification (i.e., units, tens, etc.) and manipulation <strong>of</strong> digits within<br />

<strong>the</strong> base-10 <strong>structure</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>Arabic</strong> <strong>number</strong> system (i.e., carrying from one position to<br />

ano<strong>the</strong>r). Finally, Green, Lemaire, and Dufau (2007) suggested that <strong>the</strong> carry effect may also<br />

reflect processes necessary to adapt solution strategies to <strong>the</strong> actual problem (see also<br />

Torbeyns, Verschavel, & Ghesquiere, 2002). In sum, <strong>the</strong>se data suggest that <strong>the</strong>re seem to be<br />

different sources <strong>of</strong> difficulty for <strong>the</strong> carry operation.<br />

At this point, evaluating eye fixation behaviour may supplement <strong>the</strong> interpretation <strong>of</strong><br />

response latencies and error rates when investigating <strong>the</strong> temporal dynamics <strong>of</strong> <strong>number</strong><br />

processing (cf. Brysbaert, 1995). According to <strong>the</strong> immediacy and eye mind assumption (i)<br />

<strong>the</strong> eyes tend to fixate those objects from which visual information is extracted to support<br />

<strong>the</strong>ir cognitive evaluation (e.g., Just & Carpenter, 1980; Rayner & Pollatsek, 1989).<br />

Additionally, (ii) fixation durations are agreed to reflect a reliable measure <strong>of</strong> how long<br />

processing <strong>of</strong> a particular stimulus takes (for a review see Rayner, 1998). <strong>The</strong>reby, evaluation<br />

<strong>of</strong> participants’ eye fixation behaviour may be a valuable tool to dissociate cognitive<br />

processes in numerical cognition.<br />

However, in contrast to o<strong>the</strong>r neuro-cognitive domains such as e.g., reading research<br />

only few studies in <strong>the</strong> domain <strong>of</strong> numerical cognition have yet tapped <strong>the</strong> potential <strong>of</strong> eyetracking<br />

data. Never<strong>the</strong>less, <strong>the</strong> eye-tracking methodology has already been employed to<br />

study basic as well as more complex numerical processes. First evidence for <strong>the</strong> validity <strong>of</strong><br />

63

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!