06.03.2016 Views

Thinking, Fast and Slow - Daniel Kahneman

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Samuel R. Gross <strong>and</strong> Kent D. Syverud, “Getting to No: A Study of Settlement Negotiations<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Selection of Cases for Trial,” Michigan Law Review 90 (1991): 319–93.<br />

the frivolous claim: Chris Guthrie, “Framing Frivolous Litigation: A Psychological Theory,”<br />

University of Chicago Law Review 67 (2000): 163–216.<br />

30: Rare Events<br />

wish to avoid it: George F. Loewenstein, Elke U. Weber, Christopher K. Hsee, <strong>and</strong> Ned Welch,<br />

“Risk as Feelings,” Psychological Bulletin 127 (2001): 267–86.<br />

vividness in decision making: Ibid. Cass R. Sunstein, “Probability Neglect: Emotions, Worst<br />

Cases, <strong>and</strong> Law,” Yale Law Journal 112 (2002): 61–107. See notes to chapter 13: Damasio,<br />

Descartes’ Error. Slovic, Finucane, Peters, <strong>and</strong> MacGregor, “The {r, n>: C. A Affect<br />

Heuristic.”<br />

Amos’s student: Craig R. Fox, “Strength of Evidence, Judged Probability, <strong>and</strong> Choice Under<br />

Uncertainty,” Cognitive Psychology 38 (1999): 167–89.<br />

focal event <strong>and</strong> its: Judgments of the probabilities of an event <strong>and</strong> its complement do not<br />

always add up to 100%. When people are asked about a topic they know very little about<br />

(“What is your probability that the temperature in Bangkok will exceed 100° tomorrow at<br />

noon?”), the judged probabilities of the event <strong>and</strong> its complement add up to less than 100%.<br />

receiving a dozen roses: In cumulative prospect theory, decision weights for gains <strong>and</strong> losses<br />

are not assumed to be equal, as they were in the original version of prospect theory that I<br />

describe.<br />

superficial processing: The question about the two urns was invented by Dale T. Miller,<br />

William Turnbull, <strong>and</strong> Cathy McFarl<strong>and</strong>, “When a Coincidence Is Suspicious: The Role of<br />

Mental Simulation,” Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology 57 (1989): 581–89.<br />

Seymour Epstein <strong>and</strong> his colleagues argued for an interpretation of it in terms of two systems:<br />

Lee A. Kirkpatrick <strong>and</strong> Seymour Epstein, “Cognitive-Experiential Self-Theory <strong>and</strong> Subjective<br />

Probability: Evidence for Two Conceptual Systems,” Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology 63 (1992): 534–44.<br />

judged it as more dangerous: Kimihiko Yamagishi, “When a 12.86% Mortality Is More<br />

Dangerous Than 24.14%: Implications for Risk Communication,” Applied Cognitive<br />

Psychology 11 (1997): 495–506.<br />

forensic psychologists: Slovic, Monahan, <strong>and</strong> MacGregor, “Violence Risk Assessment <strong>and</strong><br />

Risk Communication.”<br />

“1 of 1,000 capital cases”: Jonathan J. Koehler, “When Are People Persuaded by DNA Match<br />

Statistics?” Law <strong>and</strong> Human Behavior 25 (2001): 493–513.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!