20.12.2012 Views

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Apprenticeships<br />

Encourage <strong>the</strong> use of more relationship-driven<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g by provid<strong>in</strong>g apprenticeship relationships<br />

with experts. Multi-age classrooms, big-bro<strong>the</strong>r/<br />

big-sister programs, and community-active adults<br />

are perfect examples of support systems.<br />

Th<strong>in</strong>k Big<br />

Do fewer, but more complex projects, especially<br />

lengthy multi-level projects, with sufficient time<br />

and resources. Students <strong>in</strong> a science class could<br />

plan a five-year trip to Mars. The project would<br />

<strong>in</strong>volve skills <strong>in</strong> math, science, problem solv<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

research, economics, and social skills. Complex<br />

projects present more opportunities for curiosity,<br />

mystery, social <strong>in</strong>teraction, frustration, excitement,<br />

challenge, fulfillment, and celebration than shorter,<br />

simpler ones.<br />

The Importance of Context and<br />

Patterns<br />

In his book Pattern Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, Andrew Coward<br />

(1990) says <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> forms quick hierarchies to<br />

extract or create patterns. The patterns give context<br />

to <strong>in</strong>formation that o<strong>the</strong>rwise would be dismissed<br />

as mean<strong>in</strong>gless. This desire to form some<br />

k<strong>in</strong>d of mean<strong>in</strong>gful pattern out of learn<strong>in</strong>g seems<br />

<strong>in</strong>nate. Children create games that organize behaviors,<br />

and <strong>the</strong>y will arrange objects <strong>in</strong>to patterns<br />

ra<strong>the</strong>r than leave <strong>the</strong>m random. Adults organize<br />

dishes, cars, tools, sew<strong>in</strong>g articles, bus<strong>in</strong>esses, file<br />

cab<strong>in</strong>ets, and book chapters.<br />

Researchers believe this pattern<strong>in</strong>g may beg<strong>in</strong><br />

on a micro level. Individual neurons do not seem<br />

to exhibit learn<strong>in</strong>g, only groups of neurons. These<br />

95<br />

The <strong>Bra<strong>in</strong></strong> as a Mean<strong>in</strong>g-Maker<br />

networks or “clouds” of neurons seem to be able to<br />

recognize and respond to mean<strong>in</strong>gful learn<strong>in</strong>g. In<br />

fact, scientists are currently test<strong>in</strong>g models of perception<br />

and learn<strong>in</strong>g that may mimic <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>’s<br />

visual system (Bruce and Green 1990). These “connectionist”<br />

models mirror neuronal groups and<br />

synapses. Although <strong>the</strong>y caution us <strong>in</strong> call<strong>in</strong>g it a<br />

biological match, early f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs are encourag<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r areas of neurobiology suggest pattern<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g may be <strong>in</strong>nate. In a classic experiment,<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants were shown a series of draw<strong>in</strong>gs. Each<br />

illustration had exactly <strong>the</strong> same elements as a<br />

human face. But only one had <strong>the</strong>m <strong>in</strong> a coherent,<br />

human face shape and form. The o<strong>the</strong>rs had <strong>the</strong><br />

eyes, nose, hair, and mouth scrambled. To determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

<strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terest and value to an <strong>in</strong>fant, careful<br />

record<strong>in</strong>gs were made of which figures were preferred<br />

by “gaze time.” The pattern of a human face<br />

<strong>in</strong> its correct form had much more mean<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants, even those a few days old (Franz 1961).<br />

Infants as young as 10 months or less are drawn<br />

to, and can recognize, patterns quicker than nonpatterns<br />

(Mehler and Dupoux 1994). On videotape,<br />

<strong>in</strong>fants show puzzled looks when presented<br />

with scattered, “unpatterned” material. These studies<br />

suggest we are wired to pay attention to certa<strong>in</strong><br />

patterns.<br />

In tests of visual perception, researchers have<br />

shown not only that we are “naturals” at learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

pattern discrim<strong>in</strong>ation but at apply<strong>in</strong>g it to o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

models. One researcher says it’s <strong>the</strong> mak<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

familiar connections (relevance) and locat<strong>in</strong>g conform<strong>in</strong>g<br />

neural networks (pattern mak<strong>in</strong>g) that are<br />

critical to <strong>the</strong> formation of mean<strong>in</strong>g (Freeman<br />

1995). How important is <strong>the</strong> process of pattern<br />

mak<strong>in</strong>g to <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong>? Child development expert<br />

Jane Healy says, “I am <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>gly conv<strong>in</strong>ced that

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!