20.12.2012 Views

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

pursue rewards as a teach<strong>in</strong>g strategy. We knew<br />

very little about <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> at that time, and rewards<br />

seemed cheap, harmless, and often effective. But<br />

<strong>the</strong>re was more to <strong>the</strong> use of rewards than most<br />

educators understood. Surpris<strong>in</strong>gly, much of <strong>the</strong><br />

orig<strong>in</strong>al research by Watson and Sk<strong>in</strong>ner was<br />

mis<strong>in</strong>terpreted.<br />

For example, <strong>the</strong> stimulus-response rewards<br />

popularized by behaviorism were effective only for<br />

simple physical actions. But schools often try to<br />

reward students for solv<strong>in</strong>g challeng<strong>in</strong>g cognitive<br />

problems, writ<strong>in</strong>g creatively, and design<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

complet<strong>in</strong>g projects. There’s an enormous difference<br />

<strong>in</strong> how <strong>the</strong> human bra<strong>in</strong> responds to rewards for<br />

simple and complex problem-solv<strong>in</strong>g tasks. Shortterm<br />

rewards can temporarily stimulate simple<br />

physical responses, but more complex behaviors are<br />

usually impaired, not helped, by rewards (Deci,<br />

Vallerand, Pelletier, and Ryan 1991, Kohn 1993).<br />

In addition, <strong>the</strong> behaviorists made a flawed<br />

assumption: that learn<strong>in</strong>g is primarily dependent<br />

on a reward. In fact, rats—as well as humans—will<br />

consistently seek new experiences and behaviors<br />

with no perceivable reward or impetus. Experimental<br />

rats responded positively to simple novelty.<br />

Presumably, novelty-seek<strong>in</strong>g could lead to new<br />

sources of food, safety, or shelter, thus enhanc<strong>in</strong>g<br />

species preservation. Choice and control over <strong>the</strong>ir<br />

environment produced more social and less aggressive<br />

behaviors (M<strong>in</strong>eka, Cook, and Miller 1984). Is<br />

it possible that curiosity or <strong>the</strong> mere pursuit of<br />

<strong>in</strong>formation can be valuable by itself? Studies confirm<br />

that this happens, and humans are just as<br />

happy to seek novelty (Restak 1979).<br />

We have all looked for solutions to “motivate”<br />

learners. The long-term promise of better grades,<br />

pleas<strong>in</strong>g o<strong>the</strong>rs, graduation, and future employment<br />

are common “hooks.” Short term, teachers<br />

63<br />

offer choice, privileges, and gett<strong>in</strong>g out on time or<br />

early. These k<strong>in</strong>ds of rewards seem to work with<br />

some, but not all, students. A study of 849 Los<br />

Angeles County 8th graders found that <strong>the</strong>y scored<br />

13 percent higher when offered $1 for every correct<br />

answer on a national math exam. This study<br />

suggests, among o<strong>the</strong>r th<strong>in</strong>gs, that some students<br />

may actually know <strong>the</strong> material but be unmotivated<br />

to demonstrate it, accord<strong>in</strong>g to study<br />

researcher Harold O’Neill (Colv<strong>in</strong> 1996).<br />

A student can be momentarily <strong>in</strong> an apa<strong>the</strong>tic<br />

state, or <strong>the</strong> demotivation may be chronic and<br />

debilitat<strong>in</strong>g. It takes a bit of detective work to<br />

make <strong>the</strong> dist<strong>in</strong>ction between <strong>the</strong> two. If <strong>the</strong> student<br />

goes <strong>in</strong> and out of “motivat<strong>in</strong>g” states and<br />

occasionally engages <strong>in</strong> learn<strong>in</strong>g, it’s probably a<br />

temporary condition. This state has an enormous<br />

array of possible causes, but <strong>the</strong> solutions are relatively<br />

easy. Learned helplessness, <strong>the</strong> more chronic<br />

and severe demotivation, is quite different. (It was<br />

addressed <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> previous chapter.)<br />

Temporary Demotivation<br />

Motivation and Rewards<br />

Students who make it to school each day have<br />

demonstrated a certa<strong>in</strong> amount of motivation.<br />

After all, <strong>the</strong>y’ve made it to class while <strong>the</strong> truly<br />

unmotivated students are still <strong>in</strong> bed or anyplace<br />

else but school. That’s why <strong>the</strong>re are very few truly<br />

unmotivated students. The students you see may<br />

look like school is <strong>the</strong> last place <strong>the</strong>y want to be,<br />

but at least <strong>the</strong>y’ve made it to your class. And,<br />

most likely, <strong>the</strong>y are temporarily unmotivated.<br />

Why? There are three primary reasons.<br />

The first has to do with associations from <strong>the</strong><br />

past, which can provoke a negative or apa<strong>the</strong>tic<br />

state. These memory associations may be stored <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> amygdala <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> middle of <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> area

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!