20.12.2012 Views

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

Teaching With the Brain in Mind

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

work, kids, teacher, and meet<strong>in</strong>g space. Our language<br />

is a classic example of hav<strong>in</strong>g to pull hundreds<br />

of words “off <strong>the</strong> shelf ” with<strong>in</strong> seconds, to<br />

assemble even <strong>the</strong> most common sentences. This<br />

<strong>the</strong>ory expla<strong>in</strong>s why a similar word—close, but<br />

still wrong—will come out of our mouths when we<br />

are try<strong>in</strong>g to say someth<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

The o<strong>the</strong>r <strong>the</strong>ory is that memories are frozen<br />

patterns wait<strong>in</strong>g for a resonat<strong>in</strong>g signal to awaken<br />

<strong>the</strong>m. They’re like ripples on a bumpy road that<br />

make no sound until a car drives over <strong>the</strong>m.<br />

Neurobiologist William Calv<strong>in</strong> says <strong>the</strong> content<br />

may be embedded <strong>in</strong> “spatiotemporal <strong>the</strong>mes,”<br />

which will resonate and create a critical mass<br />

needed for retrieval. Enough of that thought’s identical<br />

copies have been made for <strong>the</strong> cerebral code<br />

to trip an “action switch” for you to recall (Calv<strong>in</strong><br />

1996). This computational <strong>the</strong>ory expla<strong>in</strong>s why a<br />

student try<strong>in</strong>g to remember <strong>in</strong>formation for a test<br />

comes up with <strong>the</strong> answer a half hour too late. It<br />

may take that long for <strong>the</strong> “<strong>in</strong>tention to recall” to<br />

create enough “activated thought patterns” to hit<br />

critical mass. Earlier, <strong>the</strong> bra<strong>in</strong> may have had too<br />

much o<strong>the</strong>r compet<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>formation process<strong>in</strong>g to<br />

retrieve it.<br />

Variety<br />

Our separate memory pathways are used for different<br />

types of memories. Retrieval is quite specific.<br />

Neuroscientist Jeri Janowsky of Oregon Health Sciences<br />

University says it’s common for us to be<br />

good at one type of recall, like faces and places,<br />

but not o<strong>the</strong>rs, like addresses and dates. For example,<br />

consider <strong>the</strong> l<strong>in</strong>k-and-peg systems popularized<br />

by media memory experts like Harry Lorayne and<br />

Kev<strong>in</strong> Trudeau. These systems ask you to connect a<br />

new item with a previously memorized word or<br />

103<br />

Memory and Recall<br />

number. If <strong>the</strong> word bra<strong>in</strong> was second on my list of<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs to recall, I might l<strong>in</strong>k it up to my second<br />

peg word, which happens to be <strong>the</strong> word pants. I’d<br />

visualize images of a bra<strong>in</strong> spray pa<strong>in</strong>ted all over<br />

my new pair of pants. The association is now<br />

pants = bra<strong>in</strong>. Each additional word would have its<br />

own l<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g word. But can we all do <strong>the</strong>se memory<br />

“tricks”? Most learners can use <strong>the</strong> peg systems and<br />

benefit greatly. Students who often are thought of<br />

as “lazy learners” may <strong>in</strong> fact simply be recall<strong>in</strong>g<br />

only what <strong>the</strong>y can. When your students can recall<br />

names and dates, it doesn’t mean <strong>the</strong>y’ll be good at<br />

recall<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> locations that geography requires. Figure<br />

11.2 describes <strong>the</strong>se memory pathways.<br />

FIGURE 11.2<br />

Memory Pathways<br />

Explicit<br />

Includes both<br />

short-term<br />

(5–20 seconds) and<br />

work<strong>in</strong>g memory<br />

(seven +/– chunks)<br />

ABC<br />

Semantic<br />

words, symbols,<br />

abstractions,<br />

video, textbooks,<br />

computers,<br />

written stories<br />

Episodic<br />

locations,<br />

events,<br />

circumstances,<br />

"Where were<br />

you when...?"<br />

Procedural<br />

physical skills:<br />

bicycle-rid<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

body-learn<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

manipulatives,<br />

hands-on<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Implicit<br />

Reflexive<br />

automated,<br />

nonconscious<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Conditioned<br />

Responses<br />

“hot stove effect”<br />

flash cards or<br />

many repetitions<br />

Emotional<br />

<strong>in</strong>tense<br />

emotions...<br />

from trauma<br />

to pleasure

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!