26.03.2013 Views

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

22 Animal Cognition<br />

Papers from <strong>the</strong> Tenth Regional Meeting <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> Chicago Linguistic<br />

Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.<br />

Langacker, R. (1966). On pronominalization and <strong>the</strong> chain <strong>of</strong> command.<br />

In W. Reibel and S. Schane, Eds., Modern Studies in<br />

English. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.<br />

Lasnik, H. (1976). Remarks on coreference. Linguistic Analysis<br />

2(1).<br />

Levinson, S. C. (1987). Pragmatics and <strong>the</strong> grammar <strong>of</strong> anaphora.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Linguistics 23: 379–434.<br />

McCawley, J. (1979). Presuppositions and discourse structure. In<br />

C. K. Oh and D. A. Dinneen, Eds., Presuppositions. Syntax and<br />

Semantics, vol. 11. New York: Academic Press.<br />

Prince, E. (1981). Towards a taxonomy <strong>of</strong> given-new information.<br />

In P. Cole, Ed., Radical Pragmatics. New York: Academic<br />

Press, pp. 233–255.<br />

Reinhart, T. (1976). The Syntactic Domain <strong>of</strong> Anaphora. Ph.D.<br />

diss., <strong>MIT</strong>.<br />

Reinhart, T. (1983). Anaphora and Semantic Interpretation.<br />

Croom-Helm and Chicago University Press.<br />

Wexler, K., and Y. C. Chien. (1991). Children’s knowledge <strong>of</strong><br />

locality conditions on binding as evidence for <strong>the</strong> modularity <strong>of</strong><br />

syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition 1: 225–295.<br />

Animal Cognition<br />

See ANIMAL NAVIGATION; COMPARATIVE PSYCHOLOGY; PRI-<br />

MATE COGNITION; SOCIAL PLAY BEHAVIOR<br />

Animal Communication<br />

Fireflies flash, moths spray pheromones, bees dance, fish<br />

emit electric pulses, lizards drop dewlaps, frogs croak, birds<br />

sing, bats chirp, lions roar, monkeys grunt, apes grimace,<br />

and humans speak. These systems <strong>of</strong> communication, irrespective<br />

<strong>of</strong> sensory modality, are designed to mediate a flow<br />

<strong>of</strong> information between sender and receiver (Hauser 1996).<br />

Early ethologists argued that signals are designed to<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>fer information to receptive companions, usually <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong>ir own species (Tinbergen 1951; Hinde 1981; Smith<br />

1969). When a bird or a monkey gives a “hawk call,” for<br />

example, this conveys information about a kind <strong>of</strong> danger.<br />

And when a redwing blackbird reveals its red epaulette during<br />

territorial disputes, it is conveying information about<br />

aggressive intent. Analyses <strong>of</strong> aggressive interactions, however,<br />

revealed only weak correlations between performance<br />

<strong>of</strong> certain displays and <strong>the</strong> probability <strong>of</strong> attack as opposed<br />

to retreat, leaving <strong>the</strong> outcome relatively unpredictable<br />

(Caryl 1979). Thus, while information transfer is basic to all<br />

communication, it is unclear how best to characterize <strong>the</strong><br />

information exchange, particularly because animals do not<br />

always tell <strong>the</strong> truth.<br />

In contradistinction to <strong>the</strong> ethologists, a new breed <strong>of</strong> animal<br />

behaviorist—<strong>the</strong> behavioral ecologists—proposed an<br />

alternative approach based on an economic cost-benefit analysis.<br />

The general argument was made in two moves: (1)<br />

selection favors behavioral adaptations that maximize gene<br />

propagation; and (2) information exchange cannot be <strong>the</strong><br />

entire function <strong>of</strong> communication because it would be easy<br />

for a mutant strategy to invade by providing dishonest information<br />

about <strong>the</strong> probability <strong>of</strong> subsequent actions. This<br />

places a premium on recognizing honest signals. Zahavi<br />

(1975) suggested a mechanism for this, using <strong>the</strong> following<br />

recipe: signals are honest, if and only if <strong>the</strong>y are costly to produce<br />

relative to <strong>the</strong> signaler’s current condition and if <strong>the</strong><br />

capacity to produce honest signals is heritable. Consider <strong>the</strong><br />

anti predator stotting displays <strong>of</strong> ungulates—an energetically<br />

expensive rigid-legged leap. In Thompson’s gazelle, only<br />

males in good physical condition stot, and stotting males are<br />

more likely to escape cheetah attacks than those who do not.<br />

Departing slightly from Zahavi, behavioral ecologists<br />

Krebs and Dawkins (1984) proposed that signals are<br />

designed not to inform but to manipulate. In response to<br />

such manipulation, selection favors skeptical receivers<br />

determined to discriminate truths from falsehoods. Such<br />

manipulative signaling evolves in situations <strong>of</strong> resource<br />

competition, including access to mates, parental care, and<br />

limited food supplies. In cases where sender and receiver<br />

must cooperate to achieve a common goal, however, selection<br />

favors signals that facilitate <strong>the</strong> flow <strong>of</strong> information<br />

among cooperators. Thus signals designed to manipulate<br />

tend to be loud and costly to produce (yelling, crying with<br />

tears), whereas signals designed for cooperation tend to be<br />

quiet, subtle, and cheap (whispers).<br />

Turning to ecological constraints, early workers suggested<br />

that signal structure was conventional and arbitrary.<br />

More in-depth analyses, however, revealed that <strong>the</strong> physical<br />

structure <strong>of</strong> many signals is closely related to <strong>the</strong> functions<br />

served (Green and Marler 1979; Marler 1955). Thus, several<br />

avian and mammalian species use calls for mobbing predators<br />

that are loud, short, repetitive, and broad band. Such<br />

sounds attract attention and facilitate sound localization. In<br />

contrast, alarm calls used to warn companions <strong>of</strong> an<br />

approaching hawk are s<strong>of</strong>t, high-pitched whistles, covering<br />

a narrow frequency range, only audible at close range and<br />

hard to locate (Marler 1955; Klump and Shalter 1984). The<br />

species-typical environment places additional constraints on<br />

<strong>the</strong> detectability <strong>of</strong> signals and <strong>the</strong> efficiency <strong>of</strong> transmission<br />

in long-distance communication, selecting for <strong>the</strong> optimal<br />

time <strong>of</strong> day and sound frequency window (Marten,<br />

Quine, and Marler 1977; Morton 1975; Wiley and Richards<br />

1978). To coordinate <strong>the</strong> movements <strong>of</strong> groups who are out<br />

<strong>of</strong> sight, elephants and whales use very low frequency<br />

sounds that circumvent obstacles and carry over long distances.<br />

In contrast, sounds with high frequency and short<br />

wavelengths, such as some alarm calls and <strong>the</strong> biosonar signals<br />

used by bats and dolphins for obstacle avoidance and<br />

prey capture, attenuate rapidly.<br />

The design <strong>of</strong> some signals reflects a conflict between natural<br />

and sexual selection pressures (Endler 1993). An elegant<br />

example is <strong>the</strong> advertisement call <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> male Tungara frog<br />

(Ryan and Rand 1993). In its most complete form, one or<br />

more introductory whines are followed by chucks. Because<br />

females are attracted to <strong>the</strong> chucks, males who produce <strong>the</strong>se<br />

sounds have higher mating success. But because frog-eating<br />

bats can localize chucks more readily than whines, frogs producing<br />

chucks are more likely to be eaten. They compromise<br />

by giving more whines than chucks until a female comes by.<br />

There are many such cases in which signal design is closely<br />

related to function, reflecting a tightly stitched tapestry <strong>of</strong> factors<br />

that include <strong>the</strong> sender’s production capabilities, habitat

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!