26.03.2013 Views

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

left-handers and right-handers, or monolinguals and bilinguals.<br />

These are all challenges for continued exploration<br />

whose findings are shaping contemporary models <strong>of</strong> language<br />

processing.<br />

See also BILINGUALISM AND THE BRAIN; CORTICAL<br />

LOCALIZATION, HISTORY OF; GRAMMAR, NEURAL BASIS OF;<br />

HEMISPHERIC SPECIALIZATION; LEXICON, NEURAL BASIS;<br />

SIGN LANGUAGE AND THE BRAIN<br />

—Nina F. Dronkers<br />

References<br />

Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage<br />

articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie (perte de la<br />

parole). Bulletin de la Société Anatomique de Paris 6: 330–357.<br />

Damasio, H., T. J. Grabowski, D. Tranel, R. D. Hichwa, and A.<br />

Damasio. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature<br />

499–505.<br />

Demb, J., J. Desmond, A. Wagner, C. Valdya, G. Glover, and J.<br />

Gabrieli. (1995). Semantic encoding and retrieval in <strong>the</strong> left<br />

inferior prefrontal cortex: A functional MRI study <strong>of</strong> task difficulty<br />

and process specificity. Journal <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience 15(9):<br />

5870–5878.<br />

Dronkers, N. F. (1996). A new brain region for coordinating<br />

speech articulation. Nature 384: 159–161.<br />

Dronkers, N. F., B. B. Redfern, and C. A. Ludy. (1995). Lesion<br />

localization in chronic Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language<br />

51(1): 62–65.<br />

Dronkers, N. F., J. K. Shapiro, B. Redfern, and R. T. Knight.<br />

(1992). The role <strong>of</strong> Broca’s area in Broca’s aphasia. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 14: 52–53.<br />

Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and<br />

man. Brain 88: 237–294.<br />

Geschwind, N. (1972). Language and <strong>the</strong> brain. Scientific American<br />

226: 76–83.<br />

Kirshner, H., T. Hughes, T. Fakhoury, and B. Abou-Khalil. (1995).<br />

Aphasia secondary to partial status epilepticus <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> basal temporal<br />

language area. Neurology 45(8): 1616–1618.<br />

Luders, H., R. P. Lesser, J. Hahn, D. S. Dinner, H. Morris, S.<br />

Resor, and M. Harrison. (1986). Basal temporal language area<br />

demonstrated by electrical stimulation. Neurology 36: 505–510.<br />

Martin, A., C. L. Wiggs, L. G. Ungerleider, and J. V. Haxby.<br />

(1996). Neural correlates <strong>of</strong> category-specific knowledge.<br />

Nature 379: 649–652.<br />

Mohr, J. P. (1976). Broca’s area and Broca’s aphasia. In H. Whitaker<br />

and H. Whitaker, Eds., Studies in Neurolinguistics, vol. 1.<br />

New York: Academic Press, pp. 201–233.<br />

Neville, H., D. Mills, and D. Lawson. (1992). Fractionating language:<br />

Different neural subsystems with different sensitive<br />

periods. Cerebral Cortex 2(3): 244–258.<br />

Ojemann, G. (1994). Cortical stimulation and recording in language.<br />

In A. Kertesz, Ed., Localization and Neuroimaging in<br />

Neuropsychology. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 35–55.<br />

Paulesu, E., C. D. Frith, and R. S. J. Frackowiak. (1993). The neural<br />

correlates <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> verbal component <strong>of</strong> working memory.<br />

Nature 362: 342–345.<br />

Peterson, S. E., P. T. Fox, M. I. Posner, M. Mintun, and M. E.<br />

Raichle. (1988). Positron emission tomographic studies <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

cortical anatomy <strong>of</strong> single-word processing. Nature 331: 585–<br />

589.<br />

Signoret, J., P. Castaigne, F. Lehrmitte, R. Abelanet, and P.<br />

Lavorel. (1984). Rediscovery <strong>of</strong> Leborgne’s brain: Anatomical<br />

description with CT scan. Brain and Language 22: 303–<br />

319.<br />

Language <strong>of</strong> Thought 451<br />

Stromswold, K., D. Caplan, N. Alpert, and S. Rauch. (1996).<br />

Localization <strong>of</strong> syntactic comprehension by positron emission<br />

tomography. Brain and Language 52: 452–473.<br />

Vanier, M., and D. Caplan. (1990). CT-scan correlates <strong>of</strong> agrammatism.<br />

In L. Menn and L. Obler, Eds., Agrammatic Aphasia:<br />

A Cross-Linguistic Narrative Sourcebook. Amsterdam: John<br />

Benjamins, pp. 37–114.<br />

Warburton, E., R. Wise, C. Price, C. Weiller, U. Hadar, S. Ramsey,<br />

and R. Frackowiak. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal<br />

subjects. Studies with PET. Brain 119: 159–179.<br />

Wernicke, C. (1874). Der aphasische Symptomencomplex. Breslau:<br />

Kohn und Weigert.<br />

Zurif, E. B., A. Caramazza, and R. Meyerson. (1972). Grammatical<br />

judgements <strong>of</strong> agrammatic aphasics. Neuropsychologia 10:<br />

405–417.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r Readings<br />

Benson, D. F., and A. Ardila. (1996). Aphasia: A Clinical Perspective.<br />

New York: Oxford University Press.<br />

Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and Linguistic Aphasiology.<br />

New York: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Dronkers, N., and R. T. Knight. (Forthcoming). The neural architecture<br />

<strong>of</strong> language disorders. In M. Gazzaniga, Ed., The <strong>Cognitive</strong><br />

Neurosciences.<br />

Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding Aphasia. San Diego: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

Stemmer, B., and H. Whitaker, Eds. (1998). Handbook <strong>of</strong> Neurolinguistics.<br />

New York: Academic Press.<br />

Language <strong>of</strong> Thought<br />

The language <strong>of</strong> thought hypo<strong>the</strong>sis is an idea, or family <strong>of</strong><br />

ideas, about <strong>the</strong> way we represent our world, and hence an<br />

idea about how our behavior is to be explained. Humans are<br />

marvelously flexible organisms. The commuter surviving<br />

her daily trip to New York, <strong>the</strong> subsistence agriculturalist,<br />

<strong>the</strong> inhabitant <strong>of</strong> a chaotic African state all thread <strong>the</strong>ir different<br />

ways through <strong>the</strong> maze <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong>ir day. This ability to<br />

adapt to a complex and changing world is grounded in our<br />

mental capacities. We navigate our way through our social<br />

and physical world by constructing an inner representation,<br />

an inner map <strong>of</strong> that world, and we plot our course from that<br />

inner map and from our representation <strong>of</strong> where we want to<br />

get to. Our capacity for negotiating our complex and variable<br />

environment is based on a representation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world<br />

as we take it to be, and a representation <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> world as we<br />

would like it to be. In <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> FOLK PSYCHOLOGY—<br />

our everyday set <strong>of</strong> concepts for thinking about ourselves<br />

and o<strong>the</strong>rs—<strong>the</strong>se are an agent’s beliefs and desires. Their<br />

interaction explains action. Thus Truman ordered <strong>the</strong> bombing<br />

<strong>of</strong> Japan because he wanted to end World War II as<br />

quickly as possible, and he believed that bombing <strong>of</strong>fered<br />

his best chance <strong>of</strong> attaining that end.<br />

We represent—think about—many features <strong>of</strong> our world.<br />

We have opinions on politics, football, food, <strong>the</strong> best way to<br />

bring up children, and much more. Our potential range <strong>of</strong><br />

opinion is richer still. You may not have had views on <strong>the</strong><br />

pleasures <strong>of</strong> eating opossum roadkills, but now that you are<br />

prompted, you quickly will. This richness <strong>of</strong> our cognitive<br />

range is important to <strong>the</strong> language <strong>of</strong> thought hypo<strong>the</strong>sis. Its

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!