26.03.2013 Views

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences - Cryptome

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

At <strong>the</strong> same time, selectivities in multiple mental<br />

domains must surely be integrated to give coherent, purposive<br />

behaviour (Duncan 1996). It has <strong>of</strong>ten been proposed<br />

that some mental “executive” takes overall responsibility for<br />

coordinating mental activity (e.g., Baddeley 1986); for<br />

example, for ensuring that appropriate goals, actions, and<br />

perceptual inputs are all selected toge<strong>the</strong>r. At least as attractive,<br />

perhaps, is an approach through self-organization. By<br />

analogy with “relaxation” models <strong>of</strong> many mental processes<br />

(McClelland and Rumelhart 1981), selected material in any<br />

one mental domain (e.g., active goals, perceptual inputs,<br />

material from memory) may support selection <strong>of</strong> related<br />

material in o<strong>the</strong>r domains. The description <strong>of</strong> top-down control<br />

given earlier, for example, implies that goals control<br />

perceptual selection; equally, however, active goals can<br />

always be overturned by novel perceptual input, as when a<br />

telephone rings or a friend passes by in <strong>the</strong> street. Whichever<br />

approach is taken, a central aspect <strong>of</strong> “attention” is this<br />

question <strong>of</strong> overall mental coordination.<br />

See also CONSCIOUSNESS, NEUROBIOLOGY OF; EYE<br />

MOVEMENTS AND VISUAL ATTENTION; INTROSPECTION;<br />

MEMORY; NEURAL NETWORKS; SELF-KNOWLEDGE; TOP-<br />

DOWN PROCESSING IN VISION<br />

—John Duncan<br />

References<br />

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University<br />

Press.<br />

Bender, M. B. (1952). Disorders in Perception. Springfield, IL:<br />

Charles C. Thomas.<br />

Bourke, P. A., J. Duncan, and I. Nimmo-Smith. (1996). A general<br />

factor involved in dual task performance decrement. Quarterly<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology 49A: 525–545.<br />

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. London:<br />

Pergamon.<br />

Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and Stress. London: Academic<br />

Press.<br />

Corteen, R. S., and D. Dunn. (1974). Shock-associated words in a<br />

nonattended message: a test for momentary awareness. Journal<br />

<strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology 102: 1143–1144.<br />

Desimone, R., and L. G. Ungerleider. (1989). Neural mechanisms<br />

<strong>of</strong> visual processing in monkeys. In F. Boller and J. Grafman,<br />

Eds., Handbook <strong>of</strong> Neuropsychology, vol. 2. Amsterdam:<br />

Elsevier, pp. 267–299.<br />

Desimone, R., and J. Duncan. (1995). Neural mechanisms <strong>of</strong> selective<br />

visual attention. Annual Review <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience 18: 193–<br />

222.<br />

Duncan, J. (1996). Cooperating brain systems in selective perception<br />

and action. In T. Inui and J. L. McClelland, Eds., Attention<br />

and Performance XVI. Cambridge, MA: <strong>MIT</strong> Press, pp. 549–<br />

578.<br />

Heinze, H. J., G. R. Mangun, W. Burchert, H. Hinrichs, M. Scholz,<br />

T. F. Munte, A. Gos, M. Scherg, S. Johannes, H. Hundeshagen,<br />

M. S. Gazzaniga, and S. A. Hillyard. (1994). Combined spatial<br />

and temporal imaging <strong>of</strong> brain activity during visual selective<br />

attention in humans. Nature 372: 543–546.<br />

James, W. (1890). The Principles <strong>of</strong> Psychology. New York: Holt.<br />

Jonides, J., and S. Yantis. (1988). Uniqueness <strong>of</strong> abrupt visual<br />

onset in capturing attention. Perception and Psychophysics 43:<br />

346–354.<br />

Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher Cortical Functions in Man. London:<br />

Tavistock.<br />

Attention in <strong>the</strong> Animal Brain 41<br />

McClelland, J. L., and D. E. Rumelhart. (1981). An interactive activation<br />

model <strong>of</strong> context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An<br />

account <strong>of</strong> basic findings. Psychological Review 88: 375–407.<br />

Moran, J., and R. Desimone. (1985). Selective attention gates visual<br />

processing in <strong>the</strong> extratriate cortex. Science 229: 782–784.<br />

Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: affective cues<br />

and <strong>the</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> instructions. Quarterly Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental<br />

Psychology 11: 56–60.<br />

Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric Explorations <strong>of</strong> Mind. Hillsdale,<br />

NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Reason, J., and K. Mycielska. (1982). Absent-minded? The Psychology<br />

<strong>of</strong> Mental Lapses and Everyday Errors. Englewood<br />

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.<br />

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies <strong>of</strong> interference in serial verbal reactions.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology 18: 643–662.<br />

Treisman, A. M., and A. Davies. (1973). Divided attention to ear<br />

and eye. In S. Kornblum, Ed., Attention and Performance IV.<br />

London: Academic Press, pp. 101–117.<br />

von Wright, J. M. (1968). Selection in visual immediate memory.<br />

Quarterly Journal <strong>of</strong> Experimental Psychology 20: 62–68.<br />

Wolford, G., and F. Morrison. (1980). Processing <strong>of</strong> unattended<br />

visual information. Memory and Cognition 8: 521–527.<br />

Fur<strong>the</strong>r Readings<br />

Allport, D. A. (1989). Visual attention. In M. I. Posner, Ed., Foundations<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>Cognitive</strong> Science. Cambridge, MA: <strong>MIT</strong> Press, pp.<br />

631–682.<br />

Norman, D. A., and T. Shallice. (1986). Attention to action: willed<br />

and automatic control <strong>of</strong> behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E.<br />

Schwartz, and D. Shapiro, Eds., Consciousness and self-regulation.<br />

Advances in research and <strong>the</strong>ory, vol. 4. New York: Plenum,<br />

pp. 1–18.<br />

Pashler, H. (1997). The Psychology <strong>of</strong> Attention. Cambridge, MA:<br />

<strong>MIT</strong> press.<br />

Posner, M. I., and S. E. Petersen. (1990). The attention system <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> human brain. Annual Review <strong>of</strong> Neuroscience 13: 25–42.<br />

Attention in <strong>the</strong> Animal Brain<br />

In most contexts ATTENTION refers to our ability to concentrate<br />

our perceptual experience on a selected portion <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong><br />

available sensory information, and, in doing so, to achieve a<br />

clear and vivid impression <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> environment. To evaluate<br />

something that seems as fundamentally introspective as<br />

attention, cognitive science research usually uses a measure<br />

<strong>of</strong> behavioral performance that is correlated with attention.<br />

To examine brain mechanisms <strong>of</strong> attention, <strong>the</strong> correlations<br />

are extended ano<strong>the</strong>r level by measuring <strong>the</strong> activity <strong>of</strong> neurons<br />

during different ‘attentive’ behaviors. Although this<br />

article focuses exclusively on attentive processes in <strong>the</strong><br />

visual system, attentive processing occurs within each sensory<br />

system (see AUDITORY ATTENTION) and more generally<br />

in most aspects <strong>of</strong> cognitive brain function. Our understanding<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> neuronal correlates <strong>of</strong> attention comes principally<br />

from <strong>the</strong> study <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> influence <strong>of</strong> attentive acts on visual<br />

processing as observed in animals. The selective aspects <strong>of</strong><br />

attention are apparent in both <strong>of</strong> vision’s principal functions,<br />

identifying objects and navigating with respect to objects<br />

and surfaces.<br />

Attention is a dynamic process added on top <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> passive<br />

elements <strong>of</strong> selection provided by <strong>the</strong> architecture <strong>of</strong>

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!