25.07.2013 Views

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Attitudes toward assessors<br />

Perceptions toward interaction<br />

process with assessors<br />

Perceptions toward the engaged<br />

learning activity<br />

Discussion & Conclusions<br />

Pre M (SD)<br />

Post M (SD)<br />

Adjusted M<br />

Pre M (SD)<br />

Post M (SD)<br />

Adjusted M<br />

Pre M (SD)<br />

Post M (SD)<br />

Adjusted M<br />

85.4 (8.98)<br />

87.64 (11.54)<br />

84.52<br />

89.61 (9.16)<br />

90.22 (12.09)<br />

87.45<br />

29.61 (4.99)<br />

28.31 (6.08)<br />

26.37<br />

75.55 (14.17)<br />

78.61 (11.40)<br />

81.58<br />

81.42 (8.70)<br />

85.68 (11.11)<br />

88.84<br />

24.97 (6.59)<br />

25.87 (7.19)<br />

27.34<br />

79.43 (14.19)<br />

78.86 (13.17)<br />

79.43<br />

85.71(11.99)<br />

85.57 (13.21)<br />

85.62<br />

26.03 (6.18)<br />

26.63 (7.<strong>15</strong>)<br />

27.32<br />

When compared to real identity situations, anonymity has been shown to reduce participants’ unsettled emotional<br />

feelings and restraint in their interactions with others (Cooper et al., 1998; Moral-Toranzo et al., 2007; Pinsonneault<br />

& Heppel, 1997-98; Postmes & Lea, 2000; Yu, 2003; Yu et al., 2008), or lead to deviant, harmful, and socially<br />

undesirable behaviors due to the loss of a sense of self-awareness and individual accountability (DeSanctis &<br />

Gallupe, 1987; Kiesler, Siegel, & McGuire, 1984). Nicknames, on the other hand, have been suggested to hold<br />

additional motivational value for participants by allowing them to be identified by any codes or symbols of their<br />

choice at that point in time (Yu & Liu, 2009). Prior research has found that students exhibited statistically different<br />

preferences for the three distinctly different identity revelation modes (with the majority preferred nickname modes<br />

most when authoring or assessing questions) (Yu & Liu, 2009). This study was undertaken to further examine if any<br />

comparative educative differences exist among these three modes.<br />

The current study did not confirm the researcher’s hypothesis that different levels of identity revelation would affect<br />

participants’ academic performance, or any aspects closely related to the engaged activities (including participants’<br />

views toward the peer-assessment strategy, the interacting partners, interaction process, or engaged activity). Despite<br />

this, important implications were derived by combing through related literature and closely examining the context of<br />

investigation in search for insight and explanations.<br />

A close analysis revealed several differences in structural features between existing studies and the current study.<br />

First, existing studies on anonymity were mostly conducted within the framework of computer mediated group work<br />

(e.g., electronic brainstorming, decision-making), or competitive gaming environments, whereas the current task<br />

under investigation stressed the “mutual helping” (versus competitive) aspect of interaction and for the support of<br />

“learning” (versus group work). Specifically, interacting parties in the study were expected to serve as partners in<br />

learning and communicate with the intent of providing constructive feedback for the enhancement of questions their<br />

peers generated as well as their overall learning associated with the studied contents.<br />

Another dimension separating the present study from existing studies was the constituents of the formed groups.<br />

Existing studies mostly involved temporarily formed groups of people whose relationships have not been formed<br />

before the intervention while this study examined intact groups of classmates who have known each other for at least<br />

one semester at the time of the study.<br />

The third main difference was the duration of the experiment. Existing studies on anonymity were typically<br />

conducted over a short period of time (with many CSCW studies adopted a one-shot approach) whereas this study<br />

observed students interacting online for six periods, extended over six weeks.<br />

The fourth difference might derive from different cultural settings within which the studies were carried out. The<br />

present study was conducted in secondary school settings in an oriental country where collectivism is part of the<br />

cultural norm, whereas most existing studies were conducted in western countries where individualism is valued.<br />

As aforesaid, although no differences were observed in any of the examined variables, this study’s contradictory<br />

findings to previous studies on anonymity have important implications. Foremost, the results and insights yielded<br />

from studies done on groups with primarily oppositional or zero-interdependence relationships among users (usually<br />

the case in competitive activities or computer-supported collaborative work, CSCW) who convened temporarily and<br />

interacted for a short time, will be inappropriately generalizable to contexts where continuous, reciprocal assistance<br />

70

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!