25.07.2013 Views

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

January 2012 Volume 15 Number 1 - Educational Technology ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

ased on an integration and evaluation of data and/or opinions. Therefore, during the reflective judgment stage, one<br />

is able to engage in rational inquiry and derive a reasoned judgment.<br />

Research findings established a relationship between epistemological beliefs and reasoning about ill-structured<br />

problems (Bendixen & Schraw 2001; Bendixen, Dunkle, & Schraw, 1994; Bendixen, Schraw, & Dunkle, 1998;<br />

Schommer & Dunnell, 1997; Sinatra, Southerland, McConaughy, & Demastes, 2003). For example, research by<br />

Bendixen, Dunkle, and Schraw (1994) showed that students who view ability as innate and thus fixed may be less<br />

inclined to develop and use advanced reasoning skills when thinking about ill-structured issues. Also research by<br />

Schraw, Dunkle, and Bendixen (1995) found that well-structured and ill-structured problems engaged different<br />

epistemological beliefs. Schommer and Dunnell (1997) found that the more students believed that the ability to learn<br />

is fixed at birth, that learning is quick or not-at-all, and that knowledge is unchanging, the more likely they were to<br />

write overly simplistic solutions to problems.<br />

A question that could be asked at this point is whether different research results could be obtained if socio-cultural<br />

aspects of learning were taken into consideration in the aforementioned research studies. We believe this is a very<br />

important question that needs to be investigated, because more and more people form partnerships and think with<br />

others, and thus it has become widely accepted that cognitions are not decontextualized tools and products of mind,<br />

but situated and distributed. In spite of this, research has not addressed closely the role of social context on one’s<br />

epistemological beliefs. Therefore, in this study, we aimed to remedy for this lack of research in the current body of<br />

literature and sought to answer the following questions:<br />

1. What are the elements of students’ reasoning when thinking about an ill-structured problem in solo and paired<br />

problem-solving contexts?<br />

2. What is the relationship between students’ epistemological beliefs and students’ reasoning when thinking about<br />

an ill-structured problem in solo and paired problem-solving contexts?<br />

Methodology<br />

Participants<br />

Twenty graduate students from a teacher education department volunteered to participate in this small scale<br />

exploratory study. The majority of the participants were females. The average age of the participants was 23.5 years.<br />

Instruments<br />

Epistemological beliefs questionnaire<br />

The Epistemological Beliefs Questionnaire (EBQ) was used for collecting data. The EBQ consisted of five questions<br />

measuring participants’ perceived importance of an ill-structured issue (question 1) and their epistemological beliefs<br />

(questions 2-5). The questions on the EBQ were adapted from King and Kitchener’s (1994) interview questions.<br />

Question 1 was a Likert-type question with ratings from 7 (extremely important) to 1 (not important at all). The<br />

question was simply asking each student to rate the importance of a complex geopolitical issue regarding the<br />

reunification of Cyprus on the basis of the Annan Plan. Question 2 prompted the participants to state how it was<br />

possible for two experts to express different points of view on the issue, and explain how this sort of disagreement<br />

among experts could happen. Analogously, question 5 raised the issue of whether it could be known for sure that an<br />

individual's position on a specific issue at hand was correct, and participants were asked to explain their answer<br />

accordingly. Questions 3 and 4 dealt with how beliefs should be justified. Specifically, question 3 prompted the<br />

participants to explain how two experts could justify different views on the same issue, and question 4 asked the<br />

participants to explain how they themselves came to form and justify their point of view on the issue.<br />

The data collected with the EBQ were analyzed with a three-stage rubric, shown in Table 1, which constituted an<br />

adaptation of King and Kitchener’s (1994) seven-stage model to a simpler version. Specifically, we reduced the<br />

original seven-stage model to a three-stage scheme because, “generally speaking, models of epistemological<br />

development postulate three broad stages characterized first by absolutist beliefs, followed by the advent of relativist<br />

beliefs, followed by the advent of pluralist beliefs in which beliefs are viewed as relative, but more or less defensible<br />

4

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!