30.08.2015 Views

CONSCIOUSNESS

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

2. Neuroscience 99<br />

synapses of the synapses belonging to the learned item. Activating the postsynapses belonging<br />

to the learned item without activating their presynaptic terminals will evoke cellular hallucination<br />

of an action potential-induced synaptic transmission from presynapses belonging to<br />

the learned item inducing synaptic semblance. When more than one postsynapse (dendritic<br />

spine) of a neuron gets depolarized through the functional LINKs, during memory retrieval, it<br />

enables spatial and/or temporal summation of excitatory postsynaptic potentials to evoke an<br />

action potential. The activity from this neuron propagates in the downstream network that belongs<br />

to the learned item and induce network semblance (creating hallucination of sensory inputs<br />

from the learned item). The net effect of synaptic and network semblances provide virtual<br />

sensation of a stimulus in its absence, which is memory. Since there are several suggestions<br />

that consciousness is related to some form of memory (Crick and Koch, 1998; Ramachandran<br />

and Hirstein, 1997; Rosenbaum et al., 2007), it is reasonable to formulate a framework for<br />

consciousness from semblance hypothesis. Neuronal activity from the hippocampal and cortical<br />

oscillations as well as those that are triggered by background environmental stimuli activate<br />

a non-specific set of neurons result in the formation of highly non-selective semblances<br />

named as primary semblances. The prominent one among them is named as consciousness<br />

semblance (C-semblance). Qualia can be described as a primary semblance formed from sensory<br />

inputs from a single sensory system. Secondary semblances form in the presence of a cue<br />

stimulus used in previous associative learning. Examples include memory, decision-making<br />

and path finding. Tertiary semblance occurs as a response to a novel cue stimulus and may<br />

result in more than one semblance leaving the animal with an option to choose from. If the<br />

semblances are of nearly equal strength, choosing one of them becomes a probability problem<br />

similar to that in quantum mechanics. Extent of previous associative learning and the nature<br />

of the problem (cue) giving rise to tertiary semblance may explain the existing arguments for<br />

and against “free will”. This work should be considered as unproven until it is verified against<br />

experimental evidence. P8<br />

2.2 Vision<br />

119 Inversion of the Retinal Layers as Necessary Condition for Spatial<br />

Constancy Eduard Alto (Vantaa, Finland)<br />

The retinal layers inversion means that the brain ‘looks’ at the image from the awkward<br />

direction. All the objects are turned twice: from left to right and upside-down. For example,<br />

the figure ‘5’ reflected on the retina can not be restored into its initial shape by rotations in the<br />

plane. But in the case of uninverted retina (as if the brain looked at the image from the opposite<br />

side of retina) the objects would be seen simply as turned over ones. Thus, there must exist<br />

a serious reason for the ‘awkward’ choice. The reason may lay in the requirement for constancy.<br />

If there is a single and minimal visible dot within the completely empty visual field, it<br />

remains spatially constant even in monocular conditions. In lack of any additional information<br />

there can be the only one possible way for that. Even single photoreceptor comparable with<br />

the minimal visible dot evokes polysynaptic input to a number of neurons, and each transition<br />

from one separate photoreceptor to another one changes 3D grating of exited synapses. Nevertheless,<br />

the smallest visible dot represented by these continuously changing gratings retains its<br />

constancy. It means that the synapse grating changes its shape in some special way which lets<br />

the brain know the precise direction to the dot. This three-dimensional stimulus representation<br />

allows the visual system to directly compare successive phases of stimulus form and location<br />

within the interval of approximately 1/15 sec. Moreover, this precision provides stereopsis<br />

even for monocular vision as well, as it follows from the well-known experiments with the<br />

artificial stabilized images on the retina. (Both effects were described in the author’s previous<br />

abstracts). And need for the correct view of this 3D shape dictates the direction of sight which<br />

is awkward in other aspects. As a result, there arise some new problems. Though the correct<br />

order of mutual positions of receptive fields can be reconstructed by means of chiasmatic<br />

redirections, geometric forms of stimuli within receptive fields themselves would retain their<br />

original inverted shape. The problem could be resolved by mirror transformation, and it forces<br />

us to remember the layers inversion in the lateral geniculate nucleus where the layers 4 and 5

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!