30.08.2015 Views

CONSCIOUSNESS

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

3. Cognitive Sciences and Psychology 129<br />

participants stated that they no longer worried about the problem. Comments. This procedure<br />

is effective and offers a possibility to facilitate nonconscious thought processes in a research<br />

setting. Real life concerns can be studied as well as constructed tasks. Assessing the intensity<br />

of a problem and its resolution by measuring its emotional impact appears to be practical. Participant<br />

interviews and self reports may provide information about the dynamics that underlie<br />

these cognitive conscious and nonconscious processes. P3<br />

173 James in the 21st Century Bruce Mangan (Institute<br />

of Cognitive and Brain Studies, Oakland, CA)<br />

William James stays forever new, but his phenomenological analysis of the “fringe” of<br />

consciousness, especially the feeling I will call “rightness”, has been relatively neglected.<br />

Now various strands of experimental research – from metacognition and tacit learning to<br />

the study of intuition – have confirmed the thrust of James’ account, linking a wide range of<br />

empirical findings with the most powerful, scientifically grounded phenomenology in our<br />

language. This offers a new way to integrate and extend consciousness research, and opens up<br />

what might be called a bio-engineering investigation of the mechanisms of conscious/nonconscious<br />

interaction. We can begin to explain the character and structure of our phenomenology<br />

via its function, just as we explain other aspects of our biology. This new kind of functional<br />

analysis is not hostage to functionalism in its current sense nor to the disembodied presumptions<br />

of AI. Fringe experiences are diaphanous, have no evident sensory content, and elude the<br />

“grasp” of focal attention. Yet on James’ analysis they are at the heart of conscious cognition,<br />

constituting among other things feelings of meaningfulness, expectation and evaluation. The<br />

evaluative aspect is paramount: “the most important feeling in these fringes ... is the feeling<br />

of harmony or discord, of a right or wrong direction in the thought.” (Psychology: The Briefer<br />

Course, 183.) James did not develop this point, but it throws light on some of our most puzzling<br />

and rewarding experiences: the intense feeling of rightness that apparently constitutes<br />

the “Aha!” of great insight, the zap of great art, the sense of universal coherence and ineffable<br />

disclosure of mystical experience. Functional analysis links James’ phenomenology to<br />

two basic findings of the cognitive revolution: while the relative capacity of consciousness is<br />

miniscule, consciousness is supported by vast amounts of parallel, non-conscious processing.<br />

We argue that the fringe finesses the limited capacity of consciousness by radically condensing<br />

context information. Rightness signals the degree of integration: the more tightly our<br />

conscious/nonconscious cognitive system is integrated, the stronger the experience of rightness.<br />

PL1<br />

174 Why Did the Unconsciousness Appear in History When It Did? A Jaynesian<br />

Explanation Brian McVeigh (East Asian Studies,<br />

University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ)<br />

The unconscious has arguably manifested its ghostly presence in the corners of the edifice<br />

of Western thought since the time of the classical Greeks. Nevertheless, it has not received<br />

sustained attention throughout the centuries. We must ask why, then, the idea of ‘unconscious<br />

mental processes’ was conceivable around 1700, topical around 1800, and widespread by<br />

around 1900 (Whyte, 1978). In order to explain why the unconscious emerged as a topical<br />

issue in the nineteenth century, I employ the theories of Julian Jaynes. Specifically, I<br />

propose three interlinked points. First, the real mystery is not the unconscious, but rather<br />

consciousness itself, since it often appears unnecessary for human activity. Therefore, in order<br />

to understand unconscious processes, we must first appreciate the nature of conscious<br />

interiorization. After all, if people throughout history did not explicitly theorize about what is<br />

introspectable (consciousness), then there was little need to consider what was not introspectable<br />

(unconsciousness). This brings us to the second point: how can conscious interiorization<br />

be described? This can be done by breaking it down into its constituent features: (1) metaphoric<br />

spatialization of psyche; (2) introception (i.e., interiorized perception); (3) excerption;<br />

(4) self-narratization; (5) self-autonomy; (6) self-authorization; (7) conciliation; (8) individuation;<br />

(9) self-reflexivity. This brings us to the final point: what exactly is the purpose of<br />

conscious interiorization? Answer: it is a cultural adaptation that has increased in intensity in

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!