30.08.2015 Views

CONSCIOUSNESS

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

214 6. Culture and the Humanities<br />

24 or 30 frames per second. ‘Suspension of disbelief’ is a concept in cinema theory used to<br />

describe the fact that audience members psychologically allow themselves to ‘forgive’ the fact<br />

that the images and story they are watching would, on the surface, appear to be unbelievable.<br />

If a cinematic image appears too realistic, audience members fail to experience suspension of<br />

disbelief, and therefore ‘disbelieve’ what they are watching. ‘High motion’ frame rates result<br />

in increased believability but decreased enjoyment due to the audience’s perception that the<br />

images they are viewing are ‘too real.’ We reject certain proposed theories that this is based<br />

upon cultural conditioning, and give examples as to why such an explanation is unlikely (examples<br />

include the Todd AO experiments in 50 frames-per-second film and British television<br />

drama). We propose that this phenomenon is a direct result of the number of conscious events<br />

per second in the human brain. C6<br />

327 Art, Indeterminacy and Consciousness Robert Pepperell <br />

(Fine Art, Cardiff School of Art & Design, Cardiff, United Kingdom)<br />

One of the functions of the brain and perceptual system is to categorize data from the<br />

world into discrete meaningful chunks and so impose distinctions on the world that are consistent<br />

with the biological needs of the perceiver. However, under certain conditions such as deep<br />

meditation, when afflicted by particular agnosias, or when faced with visually indeterminate<br />

stimuli, these object distinctions break down or disappear altogether. The world can then appear<br />

as indeterminate, i.e. devoid of the objective distinctions that characterize our habitual<br />

engagement with the world (a mode of perception termed ‘nirvikalpa’ in Indian psychology).<br />

I will show that artists have long understood this contingent nature of objective distinctions<br />

and tried to create works that evoke indeterminate perception by dissolving the hard, deterministic<br />

boundaries around objects. This has resulted in varying degrees of visual indeterminacy<br />

in art movements such as impressionism, fauvism, cubism, and abstract expressionism.<br />

I will discuss my own paintings, which attempt to induce a visually indeterminate state in the<br />

viewer, and the collaborative work I have done with psychophysicists and neuroscientists to<br />

investigate the effect of indeterminate artworks on subjects’ responses and brain functions.<br />

This indeterminacy in visual experience is analogous to the inherent indeterminacy operating<br />

at quantum levels of reality, according to the standard Copenhagen interpretation. I will suggest<br />

that the viewer who interprets an indeterminate image is attempting to ‘collapse’ many<br />

potential states into an actual state in the same way states of quantum superposition are said<br />

to collapse during observation of sub-atomic events. This, I will argue, suggests a role for<br />

consciousness as the process by which the inherent indeterminacy of nature is resolved into<br />

the more determinate world we experience. I will close with the claim that the experience of<br />

trying to resolve visually indeterminate states demonstrates how the conscious mind acts to<br />

bring the world into being for us. C6<br />

328 Do You See What I See: Commonalities in Synesthetic Art Carol Steen<br />

(New York)<br />

Although awareness of synesthesia has existed for over 300 years, until relatively recently<br />

scientists seldom studied it, believing it difficult to explore despite countless similar,<br />

though idiosyncratic, anecdotes. They asked can synesthesia be real when what one synesthete<br />

reports seeing has little resemblance to what another synesthete says they see when<br />

both experience the same ‘trigger’? For example, one synesthete might describe the sound<br />

of a trumpet as a biomorphic blue shape, yet for another, it would look like a red pinprick of<br />

intense, moving light. In the 1920’s, one of the scientists willing to do research on people who<br />

experienced synesthetic photisms, though only from having taken hallucinogenic drugs, was<br />

the noted experimental psychologist, Heinrich Kluver. He, among others, wondered if there<br />

were any commonalities in what people saw and asked them to draw or paint their visions.<br />

He discovered that people drew the same sorts of things and he called these basic forms of<br />

perception, “Form Constants”. However, the fact that researchers sought measurable evidence<br />

for synesthesia presented no obstacle to genuine synesthetic artists who literally perceive the<br />

world differently from individuals without synesthesia and who quietly use the commonalities<br />

they experience to create their works. Even if one is not aware of having synesthesia, these

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!