30.08.2015 Views

CONSCIOUSNESS

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

Download - Center for Consciousness Studies - University of Arizona

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

3. Cognitive Sciences and Psychology 133<br />

tions and the scale anchors differed. We determined (1) which scale correlated best and most<br />

consistently with performance (indicating awareness), and (2) whether we could detect abovechance<br />

performance in the absence of awareness and how the scales differ from each other in<br />

terms of revealing such unconscious processing. Our results indicated (1) that PAS was more<br />

exhaustive than CR in detecting conscious influences on performance, and that CR in turn was<br />

more exhaustive than PDW. Furthermore, people using PDW were more inclined to give low<br />

wagers for stimulus intensities for which it was clear from PAS that they were mostly perceived<br />

consciously, which can be explained by participants being risk aversive. Crucially, the<br />

same was true for CR: even though it fared slightly better than PDW, participants claimed to<br />

be guessing for stimuli that PAS showed to be processed consciously - something that cannot<br />

be explained by risk aversion, as there is no risk involved in reporting confidence. In fact, we<br />

found little evidence for any above chance performance in the absence of awareness. One interpretation<br />

of our results is that participants perform the task exactly as instructed. When they<br />

are asked to specifically report what they experience, this is what they will do. Likewise, when<br />

asked to wager, they think about pros and cons of different bids, and when asked to report their<br />

confidence, they consider how much they trust the correctness of the report they just issued.<br />

Thus, other cognitive processes than those specifically related to the experience influence<br />

the results in these latter two cases. One could for example wager low even though one had<br />

some confidence in order to minimize loss; similarly, one could have little confidence in the<br />

correctness of one’s response even though one perceived (parts of) the stimulus pretty clearly.<br />

For instance, if a person has a crystal-clear perception of some non-discriminative part of the<br />

stimulus, they will have to guess, but one would be hard pressed to conclude that the person<br />

did not see anything at all. Such states of “partial awareness” highlight the fact that which<br />

measure is most appropriate to assess awareness very much depends on what one intends to<br />

measure. The current results suggest that in cases where it is possible to ask the participant to<br />

report their conscious experience directly, such as perception, only PAS can be recommended<br />

to measure conscious experience. In other words: just ask people what they saw. P3<br />

3.10 Sleep and dreaming<br />

181 Motor Recruitment of Mirror Neuron Areas During REM Dreams: Performing<br />

Artists, Mirror-Practiced, Limb Motions Kristen Corman <br />

(English, Allston, MA)<br />

This work interrelates mirror systems, biological motion, and REM sleep’s correlation<br />

with dreams to explore potential links between each system’s visuo-motor simulation of<br />

movement. The neural motor activation and output blockade of both mirror circuits in awake<br />

subjects and motor experience during REM dreams open the possibility of other patterns<br />

between the two brain states and their neural correlates. Are mirror neuron areas activated<br />

or recruited during REM sleep? ( F5, F4, IPL, PF [BA 7b], STSp, STSa, and Broca, see<br />

Iacoboni; Rizzolatti; Calvo-Merino; Grossman. Procedural visuomotor learning, Stickgold.)<br />

Motor neurons are as active in REM sleep as in waking, and REM dreams are distinguished<br />

by movement (Hobson 2009). Though dreamers don’t see a perceptual experience generated<br />

from external stimuli, they do experience “fictive” movements and visions (Porte; Grillner).<br />

REM activates parietal visual spatial attention (Antrobus).The oculomotor circuit is activated<br />

in REMs (Hong, 2009). If the F4, F5, IPL regions are activated, and if REM dreams occur,<br />

is the mirror mechanism that matches observation and action during awake perception also<br />

at play, in some form, during REM dream experience of movement? The formal features of<br />

REM sleep dreams may give us a window onto what aspects of simulation are activated or inhibited.<br />

Subjective dream-reports, preparatory to a future quantitative study, probe the dreamtime<br />

motor experience of professional dancers and musicians. Do artists practice, perform,<br />

or do specific or general movements while dreaming? While mirror neuron systems facilitate<br />

simulative relations in humans (Iacoboni, 2008), performing artists practice skilled motion<br />

for hours using either a literal mirror-apparatus (dancers) or ipsilateral, “specular” (Koski,<br />

2003) limb mimicry of the demonstrator who acts “as if a mirror” to facilitate learning (dancers,<br />

musicians). Awake, mirror neurons activate four times more strongly when adults imitate<br />

finger movement “as if in front of a mirror” (Iacoboni, 2008:68; Koski, 2003). Do artists ever

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!