24.01.2013 Views

Insomnia Insomnia

Insomnia Insomnia

Insomnia Insomnia

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Sleep State Misperception 91<br />

arousals during the stimulus (19). Patients with SSM have been shown to have<br />

elevated levels of physiological arousal as measured by 24-hour metabolic rate,<br />

although the increase was less than observed in psychophysioligical insomnia (20).<br />

Patients with SSM have demonstrated increased activity on actigraphy during sleep<br />

(21). The tendency toward mislabeling sleep as wakefulness in insomnia may reflect<br />

elevated levels of CNS arousal, and frequencies that are not normally quantified in<br />

standard polysomnography may be important markers for this elevated arousal. In<br />

one study, patients with insomnia who have low EEG α frequency activity during<br />

sleep were more likely to underestimate total sleep time and overestimate awakenings<br />

compared to patients with insomnia who have higher amounts of α frequency<br />

EEG activity (22). It has also been demonstrated that increased β/γ activity is correlated<br />

with the perception of wakefulness during sleep (23).<br />

Another potential cause of SSM is the misperception or mislabeling of another<br />

bodily state such as fatigue or depression. For instance, in two recent studies, both<br />

nefazodone and fluoxetine alleviated depression and resulted in improvements in<br />

subjective sleep quality, although patients given fluoxetine showed significant<br />

declines in objective sleep characteristics such as increased number of awakenings<br />

and decreased sleep efficiency (24,25). Similarly, no group difference in sleep diary<br />

measures of sleep quality was seen between patients given nefazodone or<br />

paroxetine, despite the declines in objective sleep measures in the patients given<br />

paroxetine (26). Some patients may attribute daytime cognitive difficulties such as<br />

trouble concentrating or memory problems to a poor night’s sleep or sleepiness,<br />

when sleep is adequate and true sleepiness is not present.<br />

Smith and Trinder were able to simulate SSM in 20 healthy volunteers who did<br />

not have insomnia by causing microarousals from sleep through the manipulation<br />

of the sleep environment. The same individuals did not have any misperception<br />

when they were restudied on other nights when the sleep environment was not manipulated<br />

to cause the above mentioned arousals. Hence, the researchers postulated<br />

that the cause of SSM in insomniacs is due at least partly to the increased number of<br />

brief arousals from sleep (27). This sleep misperception as being awake also leads<br />

to the perpetuation of the insomnia because of previous sleep being perceived as<br />

wake time (13).<br />

CLINICAL MANIFESTATIONS<br />

Patients with SSM complain of either longstanding insomnia with difficulty initiating<br />

or maintaining sleep, nonrestorative sleep, or excessive daytime sleepiness<br />

that is not documented by polysomnography and MSLT. The patient must report<br />

that the symptoms were present during the testing. Unique to SSM is the tendency<br />

for patients to report virtually no sleep for days, weeks, or even years. They often<br />

vehemently claim that the essentially normal polysomnographic recordings are in<br />

error. Like other forms of insomnia, clinically significant complaints are accompanied<br />

by reports of waking dysfunction, such as fatigue that impairs social or occupational<br />

function and that the patient believes will improve with treatment.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!