04.04.2013 Views

Psychology & Buddhism.pdf

Psychology & Buddhism.pdf

Psychology & Buddhism.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

72 Edward S. Ragsdale<br />

This relational approach has echoes of Mahayana <strong>Buddhism</strong>, in particular<br />

the Madhyamika, or “Middle Way” school founded by Nagarjuna near the beginning<br />

of the Common Era. Madhyamika’s central teaching is the relational interdependence<br />

of all phenomena. This principle serves as an antidote to the habitual<br />

tendency to impute independent existence to persons and things. Both Gestalt<br />

theory and <strong>Buddhism</strong> view reality and experience as relational facts, and both<br />

view absolutism and relativism (cf. nihilism) as mistaken departures from relational<br />

understanding. <strong>Buddhism</strong> may be further helpful in revealing the full measure<br />

of this relationality, and in clarifying the moral significance relational<br />

understanding may hold. I hope that the benefits of mutual illumination of one<br />

viewpoint in light of the other might offset the obvious risks of comparing traditions<br />

so widely separated by time, culture, and institutional nature.<br />

Gestalt <strong>Psychology</strong><br />

Let us first consider Gestalt psychology. Gestalt psychology was founded in<br />

Germany in 1912 by Max Wertheimer, who joined with Kurt Koffka and<br />

Wolfgang Köhler to build a scientific psychology relevant to human experience<br />

and respectful of the human capacity for insight and value. Gestalt theorists maintained<br />

faith in the unity of science, seeking to reveal the ultimate compatibility of<br />

mental and physical realms. They rejected the standard model – common to both<br />

introspectionism and behaviorism – of reducing mental events to atomistic and<br />

mechanistic processes in mind or body, which entails a relativism of thought and<br />

value. Rejecting traditional assumptions of elementarism, mechanism, and reductionism,<br />

Gestalt psychologists developed a field approach for psychology, to<br />

address problems in perception, cognition, interpersonal relations, and value.<br />

A few words should be said about Köhler’s (1938/1966, 1944/1971, 1947,<br />

1959, 1969) work on the psychophysical foundations of value, and the role – in<br />

mind and body – of invariant dynamics as a third principle of causation beyond<br />

heredity and learning. Köhler expanded psychology’s common view of value as a<br />

subjectively imposed, ego-driven product of the exigencies of mutation or conditioning,<br />

to reveal it as a fact of nature that is neither ego-bound nor utterly dependent<br />

on the contingencies of genetics or learning (see also Köhler, 1950/1961).<br />

Value experience may depend substantially upon processes of invariant dynamics<br />

found throughout nature (see also Henle, 1977, 1985; Ragsdale, 1999). If so,<br />

reductive and relativistic explanations will not suffice. While this view invites<br />

comparison to Buddhist teachings concerning the uncreated mind, that is not a<br />

goal of the present work.<br />

Let us narrow our focus to one aspect of the wider view: that of the relational<br />

determination of meaning and value. Gestalt psychology’s relational viewpoint is<br />

reflected in Köhler’s famous statement “the whole is different from the sum of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!