11.07.2015 Views

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Cabecinhas, R. & Abadia, L. (eds.) (2013)<strong>Narratives</strong> <strong>and</strong> social memory: theoretical <strong>and</strong> metho<strong>do</strong>logical approachesBraga: University of <strong>Minho</strong>ISBN: 978-989-8600-04-2pp. 25 -45The Lay Historian: How OrdinaryPeople Think about HistoryOlivier KleinUniversité Libre de Bruxelles, Belgiumoklein@ulb.ac.beAbstract<strong>Social</strong> psychology has mainly studied collective memory as a collectively shared contenti.e., as a social representation. By contrast, cognitive psychology appraises memory withlittle interest in its content <strong>and</strong> at a generally individual level. In this chapter, I suggesta middle ground between these two approaches by presenting a new metaphor of howordinary folks think about history: the “lay historian”. I consider how historians’ approachesto the past may find parallels in ordinary people’s construction of historical representations.In order to <strong>do</strong> so, I borrow from Paul Ricoeur’s (2000) distinction between three stepsinvolved in historical research: <strong>do</strong>cumentary, explanatory <strong>and</strong> representational. I show thatthese steps can find parallels in cognitive analyses of memory: The first process can beapproached in terms of source memory; the second in terms of causal attribution <strong>and</strong> thethird in terms of social psychological models of communication. A special focus is place<strong>do</strong>n the interaction between these processes as they occur both in historical research <strong>and</strong>in the elaboration of historical memory. These parallels also highlight novel paths tofuture research. In turn, this metaphor may be used as a heuristic tool for comparinghistorians’ <strong>and</strong> ordinary people’s appraisals of the past.KeywordsHistory; Collective <strong>Memory</strong>; <strong>Social</strong> Cognition; Hindsight Bias; Explanation; Narration«We capture from the past only what, in it, has been necessary <strong>and</strong> sufficient toproduce what, today, is reality» (Raymond Aron, 1969) 1When we reflect on history – <strong>and</strong> when I say “we”, I refer to those of us who <strong>do</strong> not holda degree in history – we <strong>do</strong> not just passively retrieve representations of the past that arestored in memory. Rather, we craft new ones, building on the store of knowledge accumulatedduring our existence. Thus, the historian <strong>do</strong>es not hold a monopoly on thinking aboutthe past. Her knowledge is actually germane to psychology if, like Marc Bloch (1947/1993),we view history as the «science of men of the past».This is particularly blatant for a parent. Once, my then 6-year old daughter askedme “why did Hitler become mean?” Responding to this question dem<strong>and</strong>ed to recollectdisparate bits of knowledge about the Führer’s existence. I could select different types1 Cited by Leduc (2010, p. 712), my translation.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!