11.07.2015 Views

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

Preamble Narratives and Social Memory - Universidade do Minho

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

The Lay Historian: How Ordinary People Think about HistoryOlivier KleinThe Representational PhaseContrary to memory, history is produced to be told, often in narrative form (Ricoeur,2004). We can consider at least two functions fulfilled by such a narrative: first an entertainmentfunction (the narrative must be pleasing) <strong>and</strong> second, what I will call a “rhetorical”function. One needs to convince the audience that the proposed explanation is the correctone, a purpose that has guided historians in varying ways across time: Whereas telling historyused to be akin to literature (Burrow, 2008), literary history is often decried as «unscientific»today. There exist several constrains as to the way history can <strong>and</strong> must be told. This aspectis to my knowledge neglected in research on collective memory (with the notable exceptionof work by Laszlo, 2008). Yet, the narrative is a specific form of expression, characterized byspecific conventions. Ricoeur suggests that historical narratives function like a plot:«Narrative coherence brings what I have called a synthesis of the heterogeneous,in order to speak of the coordination between multiple events, or betweencauses, intentions, <strong>and</strong> also accidents within a meaningful single entity. The plotis the literary form of this coordination» (Ricoeur, 2000, p. 243).Thus, the plot, as a narrative form, also constitutes a form of explanation (cf. Hart &Honoré’s quote above), rendering the distinction between the explanatory <strong>and</strong> the representationalstage quite permeable (see also Burrow, 2008; Delacroix, 2010).Besides, there may be specific conventions as to which type of explanation is acceptabledepending on the event that must be explained. For example, when Jared Diamond(1997) explains the «conquest» of America by the “horizontal” structure of Eurasia comparedto the “vertical” (North-South) structure of America (which did not allow germs to spreadacross the continent <strong>and</strong> immunize its inhabitants), this may appear like an illegitimateexplanation from the perspective of an ortho<strong>do</strong>x historian. More generally, people maya<strong>do</strong>pt preferred modes of explanation, which may in part be a function of culture. For example,Paez, Bobowik, Basabe & Hanke (2011) have identified 8 forms of lay historiography(e.g., «history is produced by Great Men», «history is cyclical», «history is drawn by a superiordesign», etc.) <strong>and</strong> found that adhesion to each of these modes of explanation differed acrosscultures. Besides, these predicted the willingness to fight on behalf of one’s country. Thisshould come as no surprise: If, e.g., one believes that history is a purely r<strong>and</strong>om process inwhich luck plays a <strong>do</strong>minant role, fighting to change the course of history makes less sensethan if one believes that it is driven by Great Men. These findings point out at the importanceof considering the «top-<strong>do</strong>wn» influences of culture on the lay historian’s chosenmodes of explanation <strong>and</strong>, in turn, their memory for the past.Precisely, the focus on narration, which involves telling a story to someone, appearsall the more important when one considers its impact on memory. Variables that are specificto the communicational context influence memory for the object of narration. For example,people remember a stranger differently depending on whether they have described thisstranger to someone who liked or disliked him. This is called the “saying-is-believing” effect(Higgins & Rholes, 1978; for a review, see Echterhoff, Higgins, & Levine, 2009). What <strong>and</strong>how we talk to others shapes our memory. Other illustrations of such interactions are foundin the work by Marsh (2007): The rhetorical intention associated with a message influences<strong>Narratives</strong> <strong>and</strong> social memory: theoretical <strong>and</strong> metho<strong>do</strong>logical approaches39

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!