11.12.2012 Views

D2.1 Requirements and Specification - CORBYS

D2.1 Requirements and Specification - CORBYS

D2.1 Requirements and Specification - CORBYS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>D2.1</strong> <strong>Requirements</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Specification</strong><br />

A notorious example for anticipatory failure is represented by the anticipation of extreme events (Nadin,<br />

2005). Such examples are catastrophic events such as earthquakes or financial collapses for which often<br />

power-law type post-hoc models can be given, but which violate the principle suggested in Nadin (2005) that<br />

“a model unfolding in faster than real time [would appear] to the observer as an informational future”, in that<br />

they screen the future from the present observer. A particular challenge is, as the author argues, that for these<br />

models it is difficult to decide when predictions are appropriate because rare, but large events are<br />

distinguished by the fact that their occurrence is not just not easily predicted according to models valid in<br />

more “stable” phases of temporal development, but that the models do not even provide sufficient<br />

introspection to detect this situation.<br />

Seizures can be considered “extreme” events in the brain. In Mormann et al. (2006), a review is given over<br />

existing seizure anticipation models based on EEG data. Methods that have been reported to be more<br />

successful are based on dynamical systems models such as Lyapunov exponents, accumulated signal energy,<br />

simulated neuronal cell models or phase synchronisation, however, later analysis seems to put these results in<br />

doubt <strong>and</strong> the current state of knowledge is considered inconclusive.<br />

On the level of intentional actions, however, it has been demonstrated that EMG registers the influence of<br />

anticipation on future events; in particular, it appears in preparatory postures anticipating voluntary movement<br />

(Brown <strong>and</strong> Frank, 1987). The anticipatory movement takes place in response to an expected task e.g. to<br />

preserve balance in anticipation of a push or pull action. The combination of anticipation with attentional<br />

mechanisms is believed to be controlled by the prefrontal cortex, by modulating sensory pathways in a “topdown”<br />

fashion (Liang <strong>and</strong> Wang, 2003).<br />

This biological evidence indicates the fundamental relevance of anticipation not only to prepare both active<br />

behaviour which requires suitable alignment activities, but also from the st<strong>and</strong>point of the preparation of<br />

cognitive processing resources. This is corroborated by studies using principled information-theoretic<br />

methods (van Dijk et al., 2010, van Dijk <strong>and</strong> Polani, 2011a). In these, it is assumed that limited informational<br />

resources are allocated for the working memory keeping track of current goals (i.e. one places constraints on<br />

goal-relevant information). This minimal assumption gives rise to salient decision transition points for<br />

behaviour strategies, such as intermediate goals. This model acts as a kind of proto-attentional mechanism.<br />

In addition, it highlights the link between attention, anticipation, <strong>and</strong> their emergence from constraints in the<br />

available informational resources.<br />

An agent’s actions carried out in the context of goal-directed behaviour must necessarily reveal information<br />

about its goals <strong>and</strong>/or purposes. This information, called digested information, can be identified by other<br />

agents that have the same goal as the first agent (Salge <strong>and</strong> Polani, 2011).<br />

For the <strong>CORBYS</strong> project, the properties discussed in the last two paragraphs are of relevance. The digested<br />

information principle under which actions reveal information about the agent’s intentions indicate that human<br />

actions should provide the SOIAA architecture with clues to the intentions of the human. To h<strong>and</strong>le this<br />

possibly sparse information, it is necessary to impose additional regularisation constraints on its estimation.<br />

For this purpose, the studies of task structuring by constrained goal-relevant information (van Dijk et al.,<br />

2010, van Dijk <strong>and</strong> Polani, 2011a) offer natural approaches to regularisation.<br />

Generally, anticipatory behaviour requires the combination of abilities to predict causally driven as well as<br />

goal-directed dynamics. The first component aims at predicting a dynamics according to e.g. a “passive”<br />

physical law, the second addresses the fact that agents, such as humans, are not passively following laws but<br />

initiate behaviours with certain purposes. The “least commitment” philosophy described earlier provides a<br />

natural framework to extract information about possible future purposeful trajectories from information about<br />

129

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!