27.03.2013 Views

International Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis - E-Lib FK UWKS

International Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis - E-Lib FK UWKS

International Handbook of Clinical Hypnosis - E-Lib FK UWKS

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

62 INTERNATIONAL HANDBOOK OF CLINICAL HYPNOSIS<br />

space, only those studies <strong>of</strong> greater relevance to this clinical handbook <strong>of</strong> hypnosis<br />

are addressed herein.The reader is referred to other reviews: Crawford, 1994a,b,<br />

1996; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992; Crawford, Horton, McClain-Furmanski &<br />

Vendemia, 1998; Crawford, Knebel, Vendemia & Horton, 1999; Gruzelier, 1988;<br />

Perlini & Spanos, 1991; Perlini, Spanos & Jones, 1996; Spiegel, 1991; and Spiegel<br />

& Vermutten, 1994).While hypnotic susceptibility level is sometimes considered<br />

irrelevant in a clinical context, it has been shown to be a highly relevant moderator<br />

in many clinical and neurophysiological studies and is thus considered within the<br />

chapter.It is hoped that knowledge <strong>of</strong> these neurophysiological ®ndings will help<br />

the practicing clinician to communicate to the medical and psychological communities,<br />

as well as to the patient and his/her family, as to how hypnosis works as an<br />

important therapeutic technique in behavioral medicine and psychotherapy.<br />

Furthermore, evidence presented herein supports hypnosis being an integral part in<br />

the development <strong>of</strong> medical treatment plans for pain management.<br />

NEUROPHYSIOLOGY OF THE HYPNOTIC STATE: IT TAKES<br />

EFFORT TO BE HYPNOTIZED<br />

<strong>Hypnosis</strong> involves an ampli®cation <strong>of</strong> focused attention either towards or away<br />

from an internal or external event e.g., Hilgard, 1965, 1986; Krippner & Bindler,<br />

1974).Since the nineteenth century hypnotically responsive persons commonly<br />

report pr<strong>of</strong>ound physical relaxation for exceptions, see BaÂnyai & Hilgard, 1976)<br />

and alterations in perception following a hypnotic induction.In this physically<br />

relaxed state, they report their experiences as being more involuntary and effortless<br />

e.g., Bowers, 1982±83), yet, somewhat paradoxically, at the same time more<br />

intense and involving than in a nonhypnotic condition.Such paradoxical reports<br />

suggest a dissociation between awareness <strong>of</strong> attentional effort perceived workload)<br />

and perceptual awarenesses.If we view hypnotizable persons as active and `creative<br />

problem-solving agents' Lynn & Sivec, 1992) who can draw upon their abilities<br />

including absorption, imagery, giving up <strong>of</strong> reality testing and focused and<br />

sustained attention) during hypnosis, then the paradox is eliminated.Contrary to<br />

common conceptions in the clinical and experimental literature, recent EEG and<br />

cerebral metabolism research supports the view that hypnosis may take cognitive<br />

effort that demands further allocations <strong>of</strong> attention and disattention Crawford,<br />

1994a,b; Crawford & Gruzelier, 1992; Hilgard, 1986).<br />

EEG DIFFERENCES BETWEEN LOW AND HIGHLY HYPNOTIZABLE<br />

PERSONS<br />

In studies <strong>of</strong> EEG brain wave activity, a robust ®nding is that theta power 3±7 Hz),<br />

hypothesized to be associated with focused attention e.g., Schacter, 1977), is<br />

positively related to hypnotic susceptibility e.g., Akpinar, Ulett & Itil, 1971;

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!