28.02.2018 Views

978-1572305441

autism

autism

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Free ebooks ==> www.Ebook777.com<br />

www.Ebook777.com<br />

Sharon 65<br />

in response to theirs. She could not see into other people’s minds. She<br />

could see their facial expressions, their eyes, their smiles, yes—but not<br />

their minds.<br />

To make up for this difficulty, Sharon visualized her own emotions;<br />

for example, anger was a whirlpool that she placed in a steel box, on top<br />

of which she planted a tree. It was not that she did not feel emotion. On<br />

the contrary, she felt things deeply and experienced a whole range of<br />

emotions. It was putting those emotions into language quickly and efficiently<br />

that was so difficult. Just like Temple Grandin, if she thought of<br />

emotions in pictures, they were easier to understand.<br />

Sharon paused and looked down at her hands. This was obviously<br />

very difficult for her. I put away my pen and looked out the window. I<br />

distinctly remember seeing a lilac tree on the hospital grounds at the<br />

end of its bloom. The petals were strewn on the lawn like painful memories.<br />

My skepticism was slowly dissolving and was replaced with a<br />

growing sense of wonder and admiration. Sharon seemed to be describing<br />

the real-life experience of not having a theory of other people’s<br />

minds, of being “mindblind” as Simon Baron-Cohen, a psychologist at<br />

Cambridge who has done a lot of the research in this area, calls it.<br />

The idea that people with any form of ASD are “mindblind” is one<br />

of the most persuasive theories proposed to explain the kinds of social<br />

difficulties that people with autism experience. They find it very difficult<br />

to accurately understand other people—their motivations, beliefs,<br />

aspirations, and emotions. It is a difficulty in intuitive understanding,<br />

an inability to put themselves in another person’s shoes and see their<br />

world from a social perspective. Our understanding of the minds of<br />

other people occurs because we carry an implicit awareness that lies<br />

just beneath conscious experience. These concepts are available to us<br />

almost by intuition; it is like an automatic way of knowing. We don’t<br />

have to think about what to say after someone says hello to us; we know<br />

without thinking. Neither are we taught these concepts in a formal way<br />

by our parents but seem preprogrammed to learn them, in much the<br />

same way as children learn to use language. Our behavior, and the<br />

behavior of others, is interpreted in terms of inferred mental states that<br />

involve motivation, desire, and emotion. For example, if my spouse’s<br />

eyebrows are raised, I infer surprise. If the corners of your brother’s<br />

mouth are turned down, you infer sadness. We intuitively use our own<br />

set of psychological concepts to understand which motives, desires, perceptions,<br />

and emotions that are part of the experiences of another person<br />

are coming into play in any given social situation.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!