28.02.2018 Views

978-1572305441

autism

autism

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

66 A MIND APART<br />

For the most part, we do this quickly and effortlessly—automatically.<br />

Typical children and adolescents do have difficulty reading social cues<br />

as well; of that there is no doubt. The difference is that this occurs from<br />

time to time, not continuously, and mainly for situations that are ambiguous.<br />

The difficulties here stem from a lack of maturity; they are not intrinsic<br />

to the person. For people with ASD, these difficulties occur in<br />

situations that typical children and adolescents would find obvious. It<br />

does not occur just in ambiguous situations but constantly. Moreover,<br />

people with ASD are often not even aware they are having trouble understanding<br />

the rules of social interaction. Typical adolescents are often<br />

acutely aware of their misinterpretation when it is pointed out to them,<br />

though they may not admit it to any parent or adult in authority. The<br />

difference is that for typical adolescents, it is emotions, impulses, or inexperience<br />

that get in the way of understanding social cues, rather than<br />

a fundamental cognitive deficit, as it is for people with ASD.<br />

Young children typically begin to develop a basic understanding of<br />

other people’s mental states between nineteen and twenty-four months<br />

of age, when they acquire the ability to pretend that a specific object is<br />

something else: This banana is no longer a piece of fruit but a telephone.<br />

This capacity to elaborate and use symbols soon evolves into social<br />

play activities such as playing Mommy or Daddy with dolls or with<br />

a younger sibling. By age four or five years, children have a remarkable<br />

sense of psychological mechanisms and can interpret and predict<br />

behavior by attributing mental states to their friends, their siblings,<br />

their parents, and themselves.<br />

What is not clear is the form these skills take at different ages and<br />

how they are acquired. Some psychologists believe that children acquire<br />

psychological concepts much as they learn the meaning of grammar and<br />

words; that is, these are preprogrammed cognitive skills, hard-wired in<br />

the brain, that unfold with development and experience. Similarly, having<br />

a theory of mind may be hard-wired in the brain, but a child needs<br />

experience for it to be fully realized (just like children need to be exposed<br />

to language to utilize their hard-wired skills). Others believe that<br />

a theory of mind arises in a child from an ability to project himself<br />

imaginatively into another situation. By this account, children understand<br />

not by having a theory of other people’s minds but by simulating<br />

imaginatively what must be going on in somebody’s else’s mind. A child<br />

might think that her mother is sad by intuitively imagining how she<br />

might feel under the circumstances and given certain facial cues. That

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!