26.02.2013 Views

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Knowing More Than We Can Tell 597<br />

mathematics and mathematics educati<strong>on</strong>), I will take as starting points mathematicsspecific<br />

phenomena, and endeavour to reach into covert ways <strong>of</strong> knowing that may<br />

lie beneath.<br />

On Furtive Caresses<br />

In his 1999 paper, Efraim Fischbein complains that psychologists have paid far too<br />

little attenti<strong>on</strong> to the phenomen<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> intuiti<strong>on</strong>: “The surprising fact is that in the<br />

usual textbooks <strong>of</strong> cognitive psychology, intuitive cogniti<strong>on</strong> is not even menti<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

as a main comp<strong>on</strong>ent <strong>of</strong> our cognitive activity” (p. 12). He acknowledges the fact<br />

that the very c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> intuiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten eludes definiti<strong>on</strong>, but suggests that it is characterized<br />

by self-evidence and c<strong>on</strong>trasts with the logical-analytical. While recognizing<br />

its importance in mathematical thinking, Fischbein also warns <strong>of</strong> the potential<br />

harm <strong>of</strong> incorrect intuiti<strong>on</strong>s for learners.<br />

In her commentary <strong>on</strong> a recent special issue devoted to the role <strong>of</strong> gesture in<br />

mathematics learning, Anna Sfard (2009) also tries to douse the enthusiasm communicated<br />

by authors about the importance <strong>of</strong> gesture in mathematical thinking, by<br />

reminding readers that the use <strong>of</strong> gestures may also lead to negative effects and undesirable<br />

outcomes. Of course, gut feelings may also be wr<strong>on</strong>g, as may metaphors<br />

and analogies. Indeed, these devices are productive <strong>on</strong>ly because they are uncertain,<br />

tentative and sometimes even murky. But, as many have argued, they are necessary<br />

to mathematical thinking. Not <strong>on</strong>ly necessary for discovery, but, as André Weil<br />

writes, for motivati<strong>on</strong>: “nothing gives more pleasure to the researcher [than] these<br />

obscure analogies, these murky reflecti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> <strong>on</strong>e theory in another, these furtive<br />

caresses, these inexplicable tiffs” (1992, p. 52). He clearly covets the covert.<br />

The challenge lies in figuring out how such uncertainties eventually lead to the<br />

greater certainties <strong>of</strong> formal, finished mathematical theorems. In fact, in her commentary,<br />

Sfard also point out a lacuna in the set <strong>of</strong> articles, namely, how we might<br />

encourage the movement from gestures—what might be described by knowings<br />

<strong>of</strong> the body—to mathematical signifiers. By starting in the domain <strong>of</strong> mathematics<br />

itself—rather than in philosophical and psychological domains that theorise<br />

embodied cogniti<strong>on</strong> and gesture studies—Gilles Châtelet (1993) provides a complementary<br />

counterpoint to mathematics educati<strong>on</strong> research <strong>on</strong> both gesture and<br />

metaphor—and, more generally, <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>sequences <strong>of</strong> embodied cogniti<strong>on</strong>. He<br />

argues that mathematics <strong>of</strong>fers two modes <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>verting the “disciplined mobility <strong>of</strong><br />

the body” into signs: metaphor and diagram. As we shall see, these means <strong>of</strong> c<strong>on</strong>versi<strong>on</strong><br />

draw together a number <strong>of</strong> <strong>of</strong>ten-disc<strong>on</strong>nected areas <strong>of</strong> research in mathematics<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>, including gesture, kinaesthetics, visualizati<strong>on</strong>, metaphor, and intuiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Further, metaphors and diagrams c<strong>on</strong>stitute principal comp<strong>on</strong>ents <strong>of</strong> mathematics<br />

discourse. I examine below first diagrams and then metaphors.<br />

Diagrams, for Châtelet, “capture gestures mid-flight” (p. 10). The gestures made<br />

by the boy working <strong>on</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>servati<strong>on</strong> task are frozen in diagrams such as shown in<br />

Fig. 1. Diagrams transduce the mobility <strong>of</strong> the body; they are “c<strong>on</strong>cerned with experience<br />

and reveal themselves capable <strong>of</strong> appropriating and c<strong>on</strong>veying ‘all this taking

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!