26.02.2013 Views

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Embodied Minds and Dancing Brains: New Opportunities 325<br />

with magnitudes) (e.g., Campbell 2002b). It is comm<strong>on</strong> practice in mathematics<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>, in accord with a relatively quite recent development in our cultural history<br />

<strong>of</strong> mathematics, to view whole numbers as a “subset” <strong>of</strong> rati<strong>on</strong>al numbers.<br />

This subsuming <strong>of</strong> whole numbers to rati<strong>on</strong>al numbers, which, ins<strong>of</strong>ar as the latter<br />

are c<strong>on</strong>ceived in terms <strong>of</strong> the number line, may c<strong>on</strong>stitute a classic disc<strong>on</strong>nect<br />

between our natural biological predispositi<strong>on</strong>s and K-12 mathematics curriculum<br />

and instructi<strong>on</strong>. If so, this could potentially account, at least in part, as to why this<br />

progressi<strong>on</strong> from whole number arithmetic and rati<strong>on</strong>al number arithmetic is so<br />

problematic for learners from early childhood into adolescence and bey<strong>on</strong>d (Campbell<br />

2006c). Identifying and rec<strong>on</strong>ciling disc<strong>on</strong>nects such as these could be taken<br />

as central issues and c<strong>on</strong>cerns in defining mathematics educati<strong>on</strong>al neuroscience<br />

(Campbell and the ENL Group 2007). But how best for educati<strong>on</strong>al researchers to<br />

go about it?<br />

Tools <strong>of</strong> particular interest for educati<strong>on</strong>al researchers are EEG and ET systems,<br />

and for a variety <strong>of</strong> reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, relative to most other methods, EEG and ET<br />

instrumentati<strong>on</strong> fall within the realm <strong>of</strong> affordability. Sec<strong>on</strong>dly, they are relatively<br />

easy and safe to use, involving minimal risk to participants. Thirdly, with sampling<br />

rates in the millisec<strong>on</strong>d range, both EEG and ET are well suited for capturing the<br />

psychophysiological dynamics <strong>of</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> and thought in real time. Both methods<br />

basically <strong>of</strong>fer temporal resoluti<strong>on</strong> at the speed <strong>of</strong> thought and place fewer spatial<br />

c<strong>on</strong>straints <strong>on</strong> participants than other methods. Furthermore, as evidence <strong>of</strong> increasing<br />

c<strong>on</strong>fidence in both the reliability and robustness <strong>of</strong> these methods, many<br />

“turnkey” acquisiti<strong>on</strong> and analysis systems are now readily available, placing fewer<br />

technical burdens <strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al researchers venturing to use such systems.<br />

Eye-tracking (ET) studies have comm<strong>on</strong>ly used methods that severely limit head<br />

movement (e.g., Hutchins<strong>on</strong> 1989). More recently, less c<strong>on</strong>straining, n<strong>on</strong>-intrusive,<br />

methods have been developed for remotely measuring eye movements in humancomputer<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Sugioka et al. 1996). These remote-based methods have<br />

become quite reliable, robust, and easy to set up (e.g., Ebisawa 1998). Most instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

s<strong>of</strong>tware today can be variously <strong>of</strong>fered through computer-based envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Remote-based ET, therefore, is bound to become an important and well<br />

established means for evaluating the instructi<strong>on</strong>al design and use <strong>of</strong> computer-based<br />

mathematics learning envir<strong>on</strong>ments (cf., Campbell 2003a).<br />

With EEG, cognitive neuroscientists have developed a viable approach to studying<br />

complex cognitive phenomena through electromagnetic oscillati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> neural<br />

assemblies (e.g., Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts 2001; Klimesch 1999; Niebur2002;<br />

Ward 2003). One key to this approach is the noti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> event related desynchr<strong>on</strong>izati<strong>on</strong>/synchr<strong>on</strong>izati<strong>on</strong><br />

(ERD/S) (Pfurtscheller and Aranibar 1977). In the course <strong>of</strong><br />

thinking, the brain produces a fluctuating electromagnetic field that is not random,<br />

but rather appears to correlate well within distinct frequency ranges with cognitive<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> in repeatable and predictable ways.<br />

As previously noted, brain oscillati<strong>on</strong>s in human cortex may be correlated with<br />

mental phenomena characteristic <strong>of</strong> mathematical thinking ranging from insight<br />

(Jung-Beeman et al. 2004) to aversi<strong>on</strong> (Hinrichs and Machleidt 1992). There have<br />

been increasing efforts to tease out a “neural code” for such correlates <strong>of</strong> affect and

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!