26.02.2013 Views

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

188 J. Pegg and D. Tall<br />

Local cycles enable the thinker to compress informati<strong>on</strong> into thinkable c<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

specified by words and symbols, and linked together into knowledge structures.Not<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly that, a thinkable c<strong>on</strong>cept is in detail a knowledge structure (called the c<strong>on</strong>cept<br />

image) and if a knowledge structure is coherent enough to be c<strong>on</strong>ceived as a whole,<br />

it can be named and become a thinkable c<strong>on</strong>cept. This compressi<strong>on</strong> takes us <strong>on</strong>e<br />

step bey<strong>on</strong>d the UMR cycle to the next level where the relati<strong>on</strong>al structure is named<br />

and compressed into a thinkable c<strong>on</strong>cept operating at a higher level.<br />

The UMR cycle in categorizati<strong>on</strong> involves the individual resp<strong>on</strong>ding at the initial<br />

stage in terms <strong>of</strong> single pieces <strong>of</strong> informati<strong>on</strong>, then handling multiple pieces, then<br />

combining them in a relati<strong>on</strong>al manner. It is <strong>on</strong>ly when these relati<strong>on</strong>al properties<br />

are seen to refer to a single overall c<strong>on</strong>cept that it can become the unistructural<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept at the next level.<br />

With encapsulati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> procedures to processes to objects, we have a sec<strong>on</strong>d type<br />

<strong>of</strong> UMR cycle: a single procedure, several different procedures to achieve the same<br />

result, seen as equivalent procedures at the relati<strong>on</strong>al level before compressi<strong>on</strong> into<br />

a procept which acts as the unistructural c<strong>on</strong>cept at the next level.<br />

However, as has been suggested earlier, the UMR cycles in embodiment and<br />

symbolism may happen in subtly different ways. It may be possible to perform acti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to see the effect <strong>of</strong> those acti<strong>on</strong>s in a way that embodies the desired object at<br />

the next level, which may then shift the use <strong>of</strong> symbolism to perform operati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that give accurate calculati<strong>on</strong>s and precise symbolic representati<strong>on</strong>s. For instance,<br />

the calculus benefits from an embodied approach in terms <strong>of</strong> the ‘local straightness’<br />

<strong>of</strong> graphs that look essentially straight under high magnificati<strong>on</strong>, to see their changing<br />

slope. The graph <strong>of</strong> this ‘slope functi<strong>on</strong>’ may then be translated to a symbolic<br />

approach using arithmetic approximati<strong>on</strong>s that give a ‘good enough’ numerical approximati<strong>on</strong><br />

at any given point and algebra to give a precise symbolic formula for<br />

the whole global derivative. At the formal level, the set-theoretic definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> limit<br />

can be introduced to give a formal axiomatic approach to mathematical analysis.<br />

The global framework also formulates the way in which individuals build knowledge<br />

structures <strong>on</strong> basic set-befores that we all share and pers<strong>on</strong>al met-befores that<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sist <strong>of</strong> structures we have in our brains now as a result <strong>of</strong> experiences we have<br />

met before.<br />

There is more to learning than simply putting elements together in a relati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

way. The learner must make sense <strong>of</strong> the world through forming knowledge structures<br />

that are built <strong>on</strong> met-befores. Many met-befores are supportive. For instance<br />

two and two makes four in whole number terms and it c<strong>on</strong>tinues to make four<br />

whether we are speaking <strong>of</strong> whole numbers, fracti<strong>on</strong>s, real numbers, complex numbers<br />

or cardinal numbers. Other met-befores that are quite satisfactory in the given<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text become problematic in a later development. For instance, additi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> whole<br />

numbers makes bigger, take-away makes smaller, but neither are true in the arithmetic<br />

<strong>of</strong> signed numbers or the arithmetic <strong>of</strong> cardinal numbers.<br />

Learners who face situati<strong>on</strong>s that are too complicated for them to make sense<br />

with their current knowledge structures, or where c<strong>on</strong>fusi<strong>on</strong> is caused by problematic<br />

met-befores, are likely to feel anxious and may resort to the solace <strong>of</strong> rotelearning<br />

to have a facility for repeating procedures but without the compressi<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

knowledge that gives l<strong>on</strong>g-term development <strong>of</strong> flexible mathematical thinking.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!