26.02.2013 Views

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

Commentary on Theories of Mathematics Education

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

178 J. Pegg and D. Tall<br />

imaging <strong>of</strong> objects and the thinking in this mode can be described as intuitive or<br />

relying <strong>on</strong> perceptually-based judgements.<br />

For the c<strong>on</strong>crete symbolic mode the ‘c<strong>on</strong>crete’ aspect relates to the need for performance<br />

in this mode to be rooted in real-world occurrences. The ‘symbolic’ aspect<br />

relates to where a pers<strong>on</strong> thinks through use and manipulati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> symbol systems<br />

such as written language, number systems and written music notati<strong>on</strong>. This mode<br />

can become available to students around about 5-to-6 years <strong>of</strong> age. The images and<br />

words that dominated thinking in the ik<strong>on</strong>ic mode now evolve into c<strong>on</strong>cepts related<br />

to the real world. The symbols (representing objects or c<strong>on</strong>cepts) can be manipulated<br />

according to coherent rules without direct recourse to what they represent.<br />

Hence, immersi<strong>on</strong> in this mode results in the ability to provide symbolic descripti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>of</strong> the experienced world that are communicable and understandable by others.<br />

As an example <strong>of</strong> Fig. 1 in acti<strong>on</strong>, let us focus <strong>on</strong> the development <strong>of</strong> number<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cepts. In the ik<strong>on</strong>ic mode the child is developing verbally, giving names to things<br />

and talking about what (s)he sees. Numbers in this mode develop from the acti<strong>on</strong>schema<br />

<strong>of</strong> counting, to the c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> number, independent <strong>of</strong> how the counting is<br />

carried out, to become adjectives, such as identifying a set <strong>of</strong> three elephants, and<br />

being able to combine this with another set comprising two elephants to get five<br />

elephants.<br />

In the c<strong>on</strong>crete symbolic mode, in the case <strong>of</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>cept <strong>of</strong> number, the status <strong>of</strong><br />

numbers shifts from adjectives to nouns, i.e., a symbol in its own right that is available<br />

to be communicated to others, c<strong>on</strong>text free and generalisable. A unistructural<br />

level resp<strong>on</strong>se in the first cycle c<strong>on</strong>cerns the ability to use <strong>on</strong>e operati<strong>on</strong> to answer<br />

simple written problems such as 2 + 3 without reference to c<strong>on</strong>text, by carrying out<br />

a suitable arithmetic procedure. A multistructural resp<strong>on</strong>se would involve a couple<br />

<strong>of</strong> operati<strong>on</strong>s involving known numbers that can be carried out in sequence. The<br />

final level in the first cycle culminates in students being able to generate numerous<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the questi<strong>on</strong> ‘if 5 is the answer to an additi<strong>on</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> what are possible<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s?’<br />

The sec<strong>on</strong>d cycle in the c<strong>on</strong>crete symbolic mode for number sees the numbers<br />

operated up<strong>on</strong> move bey<strong>on</strong>d those with which the student has direct experience. At<br />

the unistructural level, single operati<strong>on</strong>s can be performed <strong>on</strong> larger numbers; many<br />

<strong>of</strong> the operati<strong>on</strong>s become automated, reducing demand <strong>on</strong> working memory. The<br />

multistructural level resp<strong>on</strong>se c<strong>on</strong>cerns students being able to undertake a series <strong>of</strong><br />

computati<strong>on</strong>s. Critical here is the need for the task to have a sequential basis.<br />

Finally, the relati<strong>on</strong>al level in this sec<strong>on</strong>d cycle c<strong>on</strong>cerns an overview <strong>of</strong> the number<br />

system. This is evident in students undertaking n<strong>on</strong>-sequential arithmetic tasks<br />

successfully and being able to <strong>of</strong>fer generalisati<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> experienced arithmetic<br />

patterns. The issue here is that the resp<strong>on</strong>se is tied to the real world and does not<br />

include c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> alternative possibilities, c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s or limitati<strong>on</strong>s. In the<br />

SOLO model, these c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong>ly become apparent when the level <strong>of</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

enters the next mode <strong>of</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ing referred to as the formal mode.<br />

The value <strong>of</strong> acknowledging earlier UMR cycles enables a wider range <strong>of</strong> ‘credit’<br />

to be given to resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>of</strong> more complex questi<strong>on</strong>s. For instance, Biggs and Collis

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!