11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Ernest Lawrence Abel (Wayne State University) Session 61Dickensian eponymsCharles Dickens created almost a thousand fictional characters, many <strong>of</strong> whom have become synonyms for types <strong>of</strong> people ortheir peculiar traits. This presentation surveys some <strong>of</strong> these Dickensian eponyms, cites their first appearance as such andprovides examples <strong>of</strong> their usage in magazines, books, and the internet.Ernest Lawrence Abel (Wayne State University) Session 67Toponymous disorders: city syndromesT<strong>here</strong> is a long tradition <strong>of</strong> naming diseases and disorders after places w<strong>here</strong> they were first discovered. This presentationexamines psychological syndromes named after the cities in which they first or primarily occurred. The city syndromes arecategorized in terms <strong>of</strong> “tourist syndromes,” “hostage syndromes,” or “miscellaneous syndromes.”Natasha Abner (University <strong>of</strong> Chicago) Session 34Deriving meaning in the VP and DP: the case <strong>of</strong> possessives in <strong>America</strong>n Sign LanguageThe POSS sign in <strong>America</strong>n Sign Language (ASL) is used in both attributive and predicative possessives. Attributive POSSpossessives display quantificational variability that is sensitive to word order patterns within the DP. Predicative POSSpossessives are subject to a 'strict possession' requirement not uniformly present in attributive structures. Based on languageinternalevidence that POSS functions as a verbal predicate and is introduced in attributive possessives as a prenominal relativeclause modifier, these patterns result from the interaction <strong>of</strong> verbal POSS structures with components <strong>of</strong> the DP and VP domains:DP-peripheral topicality <strong>of</strong> the possessor and the structure <strong>of</strong> locative predication.Steven Abney (University <strong>of</strong> Michigan) Session 46Ezra Keshet (University <strong>of</strong> Michigan)CNF as semantic meta-languageWe adopt Conjunctive Normal Form, a quantifer-free subset <strong>of</strong> first-order predicate calculus inferentially equivalent to generalFOPC, as a semantic meta-language and present a compositional system translating natural language LF trees directly into CNF.The cases in (1) and (2), which are problematic for standard DRT and DPL (Groenendijk & Stokh<strong>of</strong> 1991), fall out <strong>of</strong> the CNFsemantics without stipulation. In addition, the system handles cases <strong>of</strong> cataphora and telescoping (Roberts 1989) and other crosssententialanaphora (Evans 1980).(1) Either t<strong>here</strong>'s no bathroom, or it's in a funny place.(2) Every farmer who owns a donkey beats it.Eric Acton (Stanford University) Session 17Gender differences in the duration <strong>of</strong> filled pauses in North <strong>America</strong>n EnglishPrevious research on the English 'filled pauses' um and uh reveals that, across a variety <strong>of</strong> dialects <strong>of</strong> English, women have a farhigher average ratio <strong>of</strong> tokens <strong>of</strong> um to tokens <strong>of</strong> uh than do men. One potential explanation for these results is that, relative tomen, women tend to disprefer sustained vocalic segments in producing filled pauses, t<strong>here</strong>by opting for um over its coda-lesscounterpart at a higher rate than men. I present evidence in support <strong>of</strong> this proposal, based on a corpus study <strong>of</strong> genderdifferences in segment duration in the production <strong>of</strong> um and uh.Chris Adam (University <strong>of</strong> New Mexico) Session 20Aaron Marks (University <strong>of</strong> New Mexico)Acoustic bases for place-faithful loan adaptationLanguages may adopt either a place-faithful or a manner-faithful orientation in loan-adaptation, the choice <strong>of</strong> which correlateswith a language’s current phoneme inventory. Within place-faithful languages, t<strong>here</strong> is a specific asymmetry regarding theadaptation <strong>of</strong> imported fricatives: voiceless fricatives map onto voiceless aspirate plosives (when a contrast exists), but voicedfricatives map onto voiced non-aspirate plosives, not voiced aspirate plosives. A statistical analysis <strong>of</strong> this phenomenon in Hindi-Urdu reveals that this mapping involves a pairing <strong>of</strong> segments based on similarity <strong>of</strong> mean duration.123

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!