11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

David Boe (Northern Michigan University) Session 74Saussure’s Course and linguistic historiographyThe opening chapter <strong>of</strong> Ferdinand de Saussure’s Course in General <strong>Linguistic</strong>s (1916), entitled “A Brief Survey <strong>of</strong> the History <strong>of</strong><strong>Linguistic</strong>s,” provides an overview <strong>of</strong> what Saussure considered to be the key developments in the language sciences prior to hisown academic career and before the establishment what he felt was the “only true object <strong>of</strong> study” within linguistics. This year isthe 100th anniversary <strong>of</strong> the death <strong>of</strong> Saussure in 1913, and in this presentation, I will re-examine the relevance <strong>of</strong> Saussure’sshort history <strong>of</strong> our field, approximately one century after his famous series <strong>of</strong> lectures were given in Geneva.Elizabeth Bogal-Allbritten (University <strong>of</strong> Massachusetts Amherst) Session 17Anisa Schardl (University <strong>of</strong> Massachusetts Amherst)Expressing uncertainty with gisa in TshanglaThis paper describes and analyzes the particle gisa in Tshangla, an understudied Tibeto-Burman language. Sentences <strong>of</strong> the formgisa p are felicitous only w<strong>here</strong> some agent A has a lack <strong>of</strong> certainty about p. Identity <strong>of</strong> A (Speaker, Addressee) depends onsentential force. In declaratives, A is the Speaker. In interrogatives, A is the Addressee. We argue that gisa is a speech actoperator that indicates the absence <strong>of</strong> directly perceived evidence for p by A, thus implying uncertainty on A’s part. We relategisa to the broader issue <strong>of</strong> the division (or lack t<strong>here</strong><strong>of</strong>) between epistemic modality and evidentiality.Juergen Bohnemeyer (University at Buffalo)Words that cut across phrase boundaries – and how to avoid themSessionThe two most frequent aspect markers <strong>of</strong> Yucatec Maya, the perfective and imperfective markers, have previously been treated asprefixes that attach to the verb across what turns out to be a major phrase boundary. I propose an alternative analysis according towhich these markers obligatorily form portmanteaus with the ergative clitics. The same behavior can be observed with themorphologically unbound aspect markers. This phenomenon is an instructive example <strong>of</strong> how the appearance <strong>of</strong> exotic syntacticphenomena can be the result <strong>of</strong> deceptively simple but flawed morphonological assumptions.Juergen Bohnemeyer (University at Buffalo) Session 24Jesse Lovegren (University at Buffalo)Elena Benedicto (Purdue University)Katharine Donelson (University at Buffalo)Alyson Eggleston (Purdue University)Gabriela Pérez Báez (Smithsonian Institution)Reference frames in Mesoamerica: linguistic and nonlinguistic factorsWe test the hypothesis that language-specific practices <strong>of</strong> frames-<strong>of</strong>-reference (FoRs) use in discourse reflect the speakers’individual adaptations to non-linguistic factors such as literacy and education (Li & Gleitman 2002). Data on FoR use indiscourse was collected from speakers <strong>of</strong> eight indigenous languages <strong>of</strong> Mexico and Nicaragua and three varieties <strong>of</strong> Spanish andanalyzed together with estimates <strong>of</strong> the speakers’ literacy, education, and L2 use. Results <strong>of</strong> a generalized linear mixed-effectsmodel suggest that FoR use in discourse cannot be reduced to non-linguistic factors: a speaker's native and second language(s) arethe strongest predictors <strong>of</strong> their FoR preference.Claire Bowern (Yale University) Session 24Patience Epps (University <strong>of</strong> Texas at Austin)Hannah Haynie (Yale University)Jane Hill (University <strong>of</strong> Arizona)Catherine Sheard (Yale University)On drugs, wildcats, and eagles: loan and inheritance patterns in hunter-gat<strong>here</strong>r ethnobiological systemsHypotheses regarding the structure <strong>of</strong> ethnobiological nomenclature systems have focused on correlations with the dominantmeans <strong>of</strong> food production in linguistic communities, relating to differences in rates <strong>of</strong> lexical replacement and the presence <strong>of</strong>generic ethnobiological terms. Here we evaluate some <strong>of</strong> these hypotheses using standard wordlists for 105 hunter-gat<strong>here</strong>r andagriculturalist languages from North <strong>America</strong>, Amazonia, and North Australia. We find that loan rates in flora/fauna vocabularyare nearly always significantly higher than those in basic vocabulary, but are correlated, and differ significantly by case studyregion. Hunter-gat<strong>here</strong>r communities also show significantly higher flora-fauna loan rates than do agriculturalists.133

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!