11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

sonority influence the roundness <strong>of</strong> forms, but the effect was non-linear. The different response patterns suggest differentunderlying phonetic symbolic mechanisms for size and shape symbolism.Michelle Li (University <strong>of</strong> Hong Kong) Session 82Stephen Matthews (University <strong>of</strong> Hong Kong)Complementation in Chinese Pidgin EnglishComplement clauses are typically unmarked in Chinese Pidgin English, which is expected due to null marking in Cantonese andoptionality <strong>of</strong> complementizer in English. Overt complementizers such as to, for, that and so are attested to varying degree atdifferent periods in English language sources. It is argued that for as complementizer could have been influenced directly fromPortuguese para or indirectly via pidgin Portuguese. The use <strong>of</strong> so as complementizer can be attributed to a convergence <strong>of</strong>English so and Cantonese gam2 ‘so, like this’ and a result <strong>of</strong> the grammaticalization path: similative > manner deixis >complementizer.Karen Lichtman (Northern Illinois University) Session 39Age, ability, and awareness in implicit and explicit second language learningThe idea that children learn languages implicitly but adults learn languages explicitly is common in Second LanguageAcquisition, but most studies on the topic either only test adults, or have confounds between age and instruction. The presentstudy taught children age 5-7 and adults an artificial mini-language under controlled implicit or explicit training conditions.Verbal ability was correlated with performance only for adults, but awareness was correlated with performance for both adultsand children, and explicit instruction increased awareness for both groups. This means that child L2 learners, like adult L2learners, are able to use explicit information about language.Constantine Lignos (University <strong>of</strong> Pennsylvania) Session 32From lexicon to grammar in infant word segmentationExisting models <strong>of</strong> infant word segmentation typically assume the learner’s goal is merely to learn the lexicon and/or constraintson words, leaving the learning <strong>of</strong> broader word structure and phonology unexplored. We develop an online, unsupervised wordsegmentation model that learns the morphological structure <strong>of</strong> words it acquires, enabling learning <strong>of</strong> phonotactics and thedominant stress pattern <strong>of</strong> the language. Learning proceeds in an online, cognitively-plausible fashion that models the process atMarr’s algorithmic level. The learner’s behavior over time matches longitudinal patterns observed in infant word segmentation(Peters, 1983) and acquisition <strong>of</strong> morphology (Brown, 1973).Constantine Lignos (University <strong>of</strong> Pennsylvania) Session 34Mitch Marcus (University <strong>of</strong> Pennsylvania)Toward web-scale analysis <strong>of</strong> codeswitchingThe quantity and availability <strong>of</strong> social media content enable the analysis <strong>of</strong> usage phenomena such as codeswitching on apreviously impossible scale. However, existing language tools cannot be harnessed to identify codeswitching without resourceintensiveannotation. Using statistical models similar to part <strong>of</strong> speech taggers, we develop Codeswitchador, a high-accuracysystem for the detection <strong>of</strong> codeswitching. We use the system to construct a corpus <strong>of</strong> Spanish/English codeswitched tweets andevaluate previous claims made regarding preferred contexts and structural constraints on codeswitching.Brook Danielle Lillehaugen (Haverford College) Session 97Beyond 'sitting', 'standing', and 'lying' in ZapotecI compare positional verb systems <strong>of</strong> Zapotec languages (Otomanguean) from three <strong>of</strong> the four major branches <strong>of</strong> Zapotec:Central Zapotec, Northern Sierra Zapotec, and Papabuco. I present an analysis <strong>of</strong> these, focusing on the selectional constraints theverbs exerts on the participants <strong>of</strong> the locative relationship, such as the size, shape, and number <strong>of</strong> the figure, the elevation <strong>of</strong> theground, and whether (and how) the figure is supported by the ground. In addition to understanding the Zapotec systems internally,I seek to relate the Zapotec system to other positional verb systems cross-linguistically.Dong-yi Lin (University <strong>of</strong> Florida) Session 34The control structure <strong>of</strong> the Interrogative Verb Sequencing Construction in KavalanIn an Interrogative Verb Sequencing Construction (IVSC) in Kavalan, the matrix interrogative verb and the embedded lexicalverb share a theme argument, which must be syntactically realized as the absolutive DP in the matrix clause. This paper examines177

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!