11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Nathan A Severance (Dartmouth College) Session 5Kenneth P. Baclawski, Jr. (Dartmouth College)James N. Stanford (Dartmouth College)Interrupted transmission: a study <strong>of</strong> Eastern New England dialect features in rural central New HampshireLabov (2007, 2010) finds that parent-to-child transmission typically occurs in an “unbroken sequence,” as each new generation <strong>of</strong>children faithfully acquires the dialect features <strong>of</strong> the local speech community. Under what circumstances can generational dialecttransmission be interrupted? Can the chain <strong>of</strong> dialect transmission be suddenly broken between two consecutive generations ordoes it require more time? Our new fieldwork and sociophonetic analysis in rural central New Hampshire shows that thetransmission <strong>of</strong> traditional Eastern New England features (non-rhoticity, fronted-FATHER and other features) is being interruptedbetween two consecutive generations as younger speakers are leveling toward a supra-regional standard.Michael Shepherd (Arizona State University) Session 15Critical discourse analysis <strong>of</strong> synchronic and diachronic variation in institutional turn-allocationThis paper examines two discursive strategies primary-school teachers use in allocating turns: (1) calling on students without theirhaving volunteered and (2) soliciting volunteers then selecting among them. Studies in the 1970s found teachers used strategy 1primarily, but data from 2008 suggest strategy 2 now predominates. Adjacency-pair and functional analyses show strategy 1 canpromote equitable participation—by allowing selection <strong>of</strong> any student—but risks dispreferred responses. Strategy 2 helps avoiddispreferred responses with a volunteer-soliciting presequence, but a regression analysis reveals it disadvantages less-outgoingstudents by making how <strong>of</strong>ten one volunteers the overwhelming determiner <strong>of</strong> his/her participation opportunities.Stephanie Shih (Stanford University/University <strong>of</strong> California, Berkeley) Session 20The similarity basis for consonant-tone interaction as Agreement by CorrespondenceThis paper addresses the on-going debate over the distinction between Agreement by Correspondence and the previouslydominant theory <strong>of</strong> autosegmental feature-spreading, focusing on a key conceptual difference between the two theories: the role<strong>of</strong> similarity in harmony patterns. Using data from consonant-tone interaction in Dioula d’Odienné, I propose that sonorityunderlies the relationship between segments and tone. Agreement by Correspondence’s unique ability to make direct reference tosimilarity in determining segmental agreement makes it better suited for handling phenomena like consonant-tone interaction.Dwan Lee Shipley (Western Washington University) Session 71A cross-linguistic comparative analysis <strong>of</strong> the toponymy <strong>of</strong> Cornwall, the Isle <strong>of</strong> Man, and Brittany in FranceThe place names <strong>of</strong> these three regions will be compared linguistically and toponymically for their similarities and contrasts. Thethree areas <strong>of</strong> investigation were originally and continue to be inhabited by people <strong>of</strong> Celtic linguistic origins. The Celticlanguages fall into two groups: (a) the Brythonic group, and (b) the Gaelic group. Breton and Cornish belong to the Brythonicgroup. Manx is a member <strong>of</strong> the Gaelic group.Cara Shousterman (New York University) Session 58Speaking English in Spanish Harlem: dialect change in Puerto Rican EnglishWhile much research in the field <strong>of</strong> social dialectology has focused on African <strong>America</strong>n English and to a lesser extent PuertoRican English, the interaction between these two nonstandard dialects remains relatively under-investigated. The current studyexplores how community change is reflected in language, by examining the English <strong>of</strong> U.S-born Puerto Rican-identified speakersacross several generations who live in East Harlem and report varying amounts <strong>of</strong> contact with African <strong>America</strong>ns. This research<strong>of</strong>fers perspective on how and why urban dialects change over time by looking at prosodic rhythm—measured using the PairwiseVariability Index--across different generations <strong>of</strong> speakers.Kobey Shwayder (University <strong>of</strong> Pennsylvania) Session 17Morphologically sensitive phonology in Maltese and MakassareseIn the study <strong>of</strong> morphophonology, t<strong>here</strong> is an empirical question <strong>of</strong> when and how much <strong>of</strong> the phonology and morphology areable to access each other. I present two case studies, Maltese syncope under 1pl subject agreement versus 1pl object cliticizationand Makassarese stress shift under clitics, in which t<strong>here</strong> is an asymmetry in the output <strong>of</strong> a phonological rule to a root based on adifference in features <strong>of</strong> a nearby morpheme. This suggests an architecture in which the phonology to have at least some accessto morphological features <strong>of</strong> nearby morphosyntactic objects.203

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!