11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Ed Cormany (Cornell University) Session 17Left-peripheral interactions in English imperativesAn independently motivated, three-level structure <strong>of</strong> the English left periphery both accommodates imperatives and predictssyntactic interactions in imperatives. Collocation <strong>of</strong> clause typing and topic features on a single head (the complementizer) isresponsible for the unacceptability <strong>of</strong> non-contrastive topics in imperatives. Contrastive topics, hosted in FocusP, are acceptable.Negation may also be associated with Focus, and the fact that imperative subjects must appear right <strong>of</strong> negation locates them inFinP. This general framework also explains the ban on Wh-extraction from embedded imperatives and variations in topicacceptability in languages with different left-peripheral structure.Micah Corum (Universität Hamburg/LiMA) Session 82Meanings and functions <strong>of</strong> for in West African English-lexifier pidgincreolesThis paper discusses meanings and functions <strong>of</strong> for in Nigerian Pidgin and Ghanaian Pidgin English. The paper frames for interms <strong>of</strong> a multipurpose construction whose basic-level meaning is motivated by gestalt perception, mental imagery, and motormovements. Similar constructions are found in certain West African and Pacific languages that influenced English-lexifierAtlantic and Pacific creole languages.The study also discusses the most productive uses <strong>of</strong> for when it co-occurs with the locative copula de. In addition to its locativefunctions, the discussion focuses on associative and relational functions that are rendered by the collocation de for.Marie-Hélène Côté (Université d’Ottawa) Session 20Toward a definition <strong>of</strong> contexts in French liaison: configurations or constructions?This paper explores the factors determining the productivity <strong>of</strong> liaison in French (e.g. [trɛ]+[ytil][trɛzytil]), in the context <strong>of</strong> thecurrent debate between structural and lexical approaches, involving abstract syntactic/prosodic configurations or liaison-specificlexicalized constructions. The analysis <strong>of</strong> a conversational corpus establishes the lexically specific nature <strong>of</strong> liaison contexts andthe fundamental role <strong>of</strong> transitional probabilities: Liaison remains productive only after words that systematically appear in thesame syntactic construction, as opposed to words that surface in more varied contexts, without consideration <strong>of</strong> general syntacticconfigurations or grammatical categories. This supports a constructionist perspective, but one that differs from current models.Rebecca T. Cover (The Ohio State University) Session 8Two kinds <strong>of</strong> pluractionality in BadiarankeBadiaranke employs two cross-linguistically common strategies for marking pluractionality (verbal plurality): reduplication andsuffixation. I argue that reduplication, but not the suffix (-ra:n-), encodes eventuality multiplication: repetitiveness or repetitionfor events, and intensification for states. Of Lasersohn’s (1995) three types <strong>of</strong> distributive pluractionality – temporal, spatial, andparticipant non-overlap – reduplication can express only the first. In contrast, -ra:n- encodes meanings not involvingmultiplication: it can convey distributivity across space or participants, as well as non-distributive readings, including unexpectedevent realization and inchoativity. Although such readings seem distant from event plurality, they are common readings <strong>of</strong>pluractional markers cross-linguistically (Cusic 1981:74-81).Megan Crowhurst (University <strong>of</strong> Texas at Austin) Session 97Amador Teodocio Olivares (CEDELIO)An experimental study <strong>of</strong> rhythmic grouping among speakers <strong>of</strong> Betaza ZapotecBased on early psychoacoustic studies <strong>of</strong> listeners¹ rhythmic grouping preferences (Bolton 1894), Hayes (1995) identifies twoprinciples, known collectively as the Iambic-Trochaic Law (ITL): elements contrasting in intensity naturally form prominenceinitialgroupings, w<strong>here</strong>as elements contrasting in duration naturally form prominence-final groupings. Hayes invokes the ITL insupport <strong>of</strong> his restrictive prosodic foot typology, imputing broad validity to these principles. However, recent experimental studieshave provided only limited crosslinguistic support for the ITL. We report on a experimental study <strong>of</strong> the influence <strong>of</strong> duration andintensity on the subjective grouping <strong>of</strong> rhythmically alternating speech-like sequences among native speakers <strong>of</strong> Betaza Zapotec.144

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!