11.07.2015 Views

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

here - Linguistic Society of America

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Dan Michel (University <strong>of</strong> California, San Diego) Session 37Individual on-line processing differences are not necessarily reflected in <strong>of</strong>f-line acceptability judgmentsRecent debates concerning island phenomena being due to grammatical syntactic properties (Sprouse, Wagers and Philips2012a,b) or processing factors (H<strong>of</strong>meister, Staum Casasanto & Sag 2012a,b) have highlighted a need for a focused examinationinto the relationship between online processing data and <strong>of</strong>fline acceptability judgment data. The current study compares onlineand <strong>of</strong>fline results <strong>of</strong> whether-islands by high and low working memory span (WMS) participants. High WMS participants didprocess island violations more easily, but didn’t rate the same sentences differently than low WMS participants did, indicating t<strong>here</strong>lationship between WMS and online processing is not straightforwardly reflected in acceptability judgments.Elena Mihas (James Cook University) Session 24Composite gesture-ideophone utterances in the Ashéninka Perené ‘community <strong>of</strong> practice’, an Amazonian Arawak society fromCentral-Eastern PeruBased on extensive fieldwork among Ashéninka Perené Arawaks, this study is a preliminary report on gesture-ideophoneutterances, with special focus on the meaning and functions <strong>of</strong> depictive-gesture-ideophone couplings. The collected video corpusshows that ideophones co-occur with depictive, pointing, emblematic, and interactive (beats) gestures. The data are alsoillustrative <strong>of</strong> Streeck’s (2009) vision <strong>of</strong> the hands’ involvement in meaning-making, i.e. that some <strong>of</strong> the ways in which depictivegestures evoke the world ascend from a basic set <strong>of</strong> everyday activities <strong>of</strong> hands in the world, within particular ecological andcultural settings.Serge Minor (University <strong>of</strong> Tromsø) Session 46Phi-features as complex indicesI propose that the index <strong>of</strong> any pronoun should be construed as a set consisting <strong>of</strong> the pronoun’s phi-features, and a numericvalue, which is assigned freely to all pronouns. All unbound pronouns and traces are uniformly interpreted by applying theassignment function to this complex index, and the semantic contribution <strong>of</strong> phi-features is incorporated into the definition <strong>of</strong> theassignment function. I argue that this approach has several advantages with respect to the interpretation <strong>of</strong> both bound and freepronouns, compared to the presuppositional treatment <strong>of</strong> phi-features proposed by Cooper (1983).Serge Minor (University <strong>of</strong> Tromsø) Session 17Natalia Mitr<strong>of</strong>anova (University <strong>of</strong> Tromsø)Low Locatives and the function <strong>of</strong> verbal prefixes in RussianWe argue for the existence <strong>of</strong> two types <strong>of</strong> Locative modifiers in Russian: High and Low. High Locatives modify the whole VP,and hence can only combine with stative verbs or non-statives, which do not involve spatial dislocation <strong>of</strong> the theme. LowLocatives, on the other hand, can be merged within the decomposed VP, modifying the Result State in a complex event structure.Hence they are compatible with change-<strong>of</strong>-state verbs involving dislocation. We then use Low Locatives to probe into the eventstructure <strong>of</strong> motion verbs, and the function <strong>of</strong> spatial verbal prefixes in Russian.Marianne Mithun (University <strong>of</strong> California, Santa Barbara) Session 96Contact among relatives: challenges and benefitsNot all language features are equally susceptible to contact effects. Transfer depends on bilinguals establishing equivalencesbetween elements <strong>of</strong> their languages, and morphology is notoriously inaccessible to speaker consciousness. It is also w<strong>here</strong>languages differ most. The most similar languages morphologically are related, which presents challenges: distinguishing contacteffects from common inheritance and drift. Here effects on complex morphology are examined in Tuscarora, whose speakersrejoined their Iroquoian relatives in the 18th century after centuries <strong>of</strong> independent development. Significant effects can beidentified, not only in vocabulary but also bound lexical and grammatical morphemes and their meanings, and grammaticaldevelopments.Susanne Mohr (University <strong>of</strong> Cologne) Session 16Anne-Maria Fehn (Humboldt University <strong>of</strong> Berlin)Phonology <strong>of</strong> hunting signs in two Kalahari Khoe-speaking groups (Ts’ixa and ||Ani)Only recently, the hunting signs <strong>of</strong> certain Kalahari Khoe-speaking groups in Southern Africa have come to scientific attention.Our preliminary linguistic analysis reveals that they present alternate sign systems similar to those described for Aboriginalcommunities in Australia, for example. The current paper presents a first phonological analysis <strong>of</strong> the hunting signs used among183

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!