03.06.2013 Views

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

When investigating unique significant correlations (second-order partial correlations) between ‗FoI‘, ‗ideals‘ or ‗CT‘<br />

with typicality, we found that only ‗FoI‘ displayed a significant unique correlation with ‗exemplar goodness‘. There<br />

was also a marginally significant unique correlation between ‗FoI‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗output dominance‘ as well. ‗Ideals‘ did not<br />

display unique significant correlation with ‗exemplar goodness‘ or ‗output dominance‘. ‗FoI‘ being the most<br />

significantly correlated variable with ‗output dominance‘ is also consistent with Barsalou (1985).<br />

A regression analysis was performed on ‗exemplar goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗output dominance‘ (dependent variables) <strong>and</strong> ‗FoI‘<br />

(independent variable). ‗FoI‘ was found to make a significant, appreciable contribution to predicting ‗exemplar<br />

goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> a significant, although not appreciable contribution to predicting ‗output dominance‘.<br />

Experts‘ <strong>and</strong> Novices‘ Results<br />

Experts<br />

‗FoI‘ is the only variable which has a significant correlation with ‗exemplar goodness‘. Although not significant, there<br />

is even a negative association between ‗output dominance‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗ideals‘. Second-order partial correlations reveal that<br />

‗FoI‘ is the only variable which has significant unique variance with ‗exemplar goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> a marginally significant<br />

unique variance with ‗output dominance‘.<br />

Regression analysis was performed on ‗exemplar goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗output dominance‘ (dependent variables) <strong>and</strong> ‗FoI‘<br />

(independent variable) for experts. ‗FoI‘ was found to make a significant, appreciable, contribution to predicting<br />

‗exemplar goodness‘. However, it was not significant in the contribution to predicting ‗output dominance‘.<br />

Novices<br />

Interestingly, ‗ideals‘ is the only variable to have a significant correlation with ‗output dominance‘ for novices. The lack<br />

of significant correlation of ‗output dominance‘ with ‗FoI‘ (frequency of instantiation) suggests novices may think of<br />

‗ideal‘ sponsors when generating the list (although what these ‗ideals‘ are can only be speculated). ‗FoI‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗central<br />

tendency‘ are also significantly associated with ‗exemplar goodness‘. Second-order partial correlations reveal that the<br />

only variable to have unique significant association with ‗exemplar goodness‘ was ‗ideals‘. It is also marginally<br />

significantly associated with ‗output dominance‘.<br />

Regression analysis was performed on ‗exemplar goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗output dominance‘ (dependent variables) <strong>and</strong> ‗ideals‘<br />

(independent variable) for novices. ‗Ideals‘ was found to make a significant, although not appreciable contribution to<br />

predicting ‗exemplar goodness‘ <strong>and</strong> ‗output dominance‘.<br />

Generation of Members in the Category ‗Sponsorship‘<br />

A one-way ANOVA, using the median split for expertise, found there to be no significant difference between either<br />

group for the number of sponsors generated. Kendall‘s coefficient of concordance also shows that there is a significant<br />

ranking agreement between novices, experts, <strong>and</strong> their combined scores. A one-way ANOVA was even computed on<br />

the original list, where experts <strong>and</strong> novices generated br<strong>and</strong>s, which never made it to the final category of ‗sponsorship‘.<br />

This too found there to be no significant difference between experts <strong>and</strong> novices.<br />

Conclusions<br />

This study suggests that a goal-derived category of ‗sponsorship‘ exists <strong>and</strong> that sport expertise can be an important<br />

factor for the criteria chosen for sponsor typicality. ‗Ideals‘ was found to have a significant unique correlation with<br />

typicality for novices but not experts. Assuming that novice subjects base their ‗ideal‘ judgments of sponsors on some<br />

form of (relevancy) congruity basis (cf. Fleck <strong>and</strong> Quester, 2007), this would support previous sponsorship literature,<br />

which has found a positive relationship between constructs such as ‗match-ups‘ or ‗fit‘ <strong>and</strong> br<strong>and</strong> evaluations. This has<br />

previously led to academics <strong>and</strong> practitioners articulating ‗fit‘ communication messages between the br<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> the<br />

sponsee/event, which can often be a difficult task for many products. However, experts were found to judge typicality<br />

by ‗frequency of instantiation‘ (FoI), which suggests that repeated (<strong>and</strong> long-term) exposure to the sponsoring br<strong>and</strong><br />

will indeed produce more favourable typicality judgments for those consumers who follow sport more often, which is<br />

usually the target market a br<strong>and</strong> aims for when undertaking a sponsorship arrangement. Subjects‘ responses to<br />

sponsorship usually involve long-term memory even though short-term responses are usually measured in a laboratory<br />

setting (Cornwell et al., 2000). The importance of ‗FoI‘ for ‗experts‘ in this study not only indicates that the measures<br />

used were appropriate but also suggests that long-term memory was accessed by some subjects, further contributing to<br />

our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of sponsorship research. The results should give both sponsoring br<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> sports properties some<br />

confidence that, otherwise-incongruent br<strong>and</strong>s can improve their sponsorship typicality ratings by taking a long-term<br />

view of the sponsorship deal. It also suggests that articulating a message about the length of time a br<strong>and</strong> has been<br />

associated with sponsorship could enhance the connection between the sponsor <strong>and</strong> sponsee/event, subsequently<br />

increasing sponsor typicality ratings. This is because consumers may start to believe a br<strong>and</strong> has been sponsoring a<br />

specific event for longer than it has because of specific communication about other previous sponsorships. Articulating<br />

this message is often a lot easier than a ‗fit‘ or ‗match-up‘ communication for many br<strong>and</strong>s. Sponsorship residual effects<br />

should also be increase using this type of communication message as well.<br />

219

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!