03.06.2013 Views

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

Brand, Identity and Reputation: Exploring, Creating New Realities ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Do Prototypical <strong>Br<strong>and</strong></strong>s Facilitate or Impede the Introduction of Novel Extensions?<br />

Frank Goedertier, Vlerick Leuven Gent Management School, Belgium<br />

Niraj Dawar, Ivey Business School, Canada<br />

Maggie Geuens, Gent University, Belgium<br />

Bert Weijters, Vlerick Leuven Gent Management School, Belgium<br />

Novel products, defined as unique innovations that are clearly distinct from other available alternatives, tend to have<br />

high failure rates (Sethi <strong>and</strong> Iqbal, 2008). But successful novel products are often more profitable than incremental<br />

innovations (Day, 2006), providing firms with an incentive to attempt novel innovation. Novel product failure is often<br />

due to lack of consumer acceptance, rather than technological or product shortcomings (Dewar <strong>and</strong> Dutton, 1986). To<br />

increase the acceptance of novel products, innovating firms may use a br<strong>and</strong> extension strategy - launching new<br />

products under br<strong>and</strong> names that are already familiar to consumers. Benefits of this strategy include immediate<br />

consumer awareness <strong>and</strong> a potential transfer of relevant br<strong>and</strong> associations (Tauber, 1988; Aaker, 1990).<br />

Prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s - those perceived by consumers to be most representative of their product category (Nedungadi <strong>and</strong><br />

Hutchinson, 1985) - are among the most familiar in any category. Prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s are, by definition, also strongly<br />

anchored in the product category of which they are the most representative <strong>and</strong> central exemplar (Nedungadi <strong>and</strong><br />

Hutchinson 1985). Researchers have argued that category anchoring may facilitate consumer acceptance of extensions<br />

in categories close to the core category associated with the prototypical br<strong>and</strong> due to br<strong>and</strong>-category fit (i.e. the<br />

extension‘s perceived similarity to the parent br<strong>and</strong> (e.g. Aaker <strong>and</strong> Keller, 1990), but may hinder acceptance of distant<br />

extensions (Keller, 2008). However, past br<strong>and</strong> extension research has focused on products that are new-to-the-br<strong>and</strong>,<br />

but not new-to-the-market. Since novel products by definition depart from consumers‘ current category representations,<br />

the strong category anchoring of prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s may hinder, rather than help acceptance of both for distant <strong>and</strong><br />

close extensions.<br />

However, in the context of novel extensions, other features of prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s might strongly influence their effect<br />

on consumer acceptance. In addition to being highly familiar to consumers, <strong>and</strong> strongly anchored in their base<br />

category, prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s tend to also carry strong associations of quality <strong>and</strong> trustworthiness (Aaker <strong>and</strong> Keller<br />

1990; Loken <strong>and</strong> Ward, 1990). In three studies we examine whether these trust associations can overcome consumers‘<br />

perceived uncertainty about novel extensions, <strong>and</strong> whether these might offset a potential fit disadvantage.<br />

Study 1<br />

The objective of study 1 was to generate a set of br<strong>and</strong> stimuli that vary in br<strong>and</strong> prototypicality but have similar<br />

awareness levels. Based on a pre-test (N = 28) 12 product categories, each associated by respondents with five to seven<br />

familiar br<strong>and</strong>s, were selected for inclusion in the online main survey (n=235; 49% male; 25-65 years), <strong>and</strong>12 br<strong>and</strong>s<br />

nested in four product categories were retained for further analyses: SUVs (L<strong>and</strong> Rover, Toyota, Opel), PCs: (Siemens,<br />

Sony, IBM), Ketchup (Devos-Lemmens, Calvé, Heinz), <strong>and</strong> Photo cameras (Canon, HP, Sony). Study 1 also serves as a<br />

preliminary investigation of the effect of br<strong>and</strong> prototypicality on the acceptance of novel extensions. Categorization<br />

theory suggests a two-step br<strong>and</strong> extension evaluation process (Boush <strong>and</strong> Loken, 1991), wherein Step 1 consists of an<br />

assessment of the fit between the parent br<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> the extension product, <strong>and</strong> the second entails attitude transfer from<br />

the br<strong>and</strong> to the extension product. In Study 1, we investigate whether br<strong>and</strong> prototypicality facilitates or hinders the<br />

first step of this process for close novel extensions. A pre-test focus group (N = 8) identified a realistic, fictitious novel<br />

(close) product for each of the product categories identified in the first pre-test. These innovation stimuli were included<br />

in the main survey described above. Survey results show a significant positive effect of br<strong>and</strong> prototypicality on<br />

perceived br<strong>and</strong>-product fit: B = .64 (t = 17.82, p < .001); br<strong>and</strong> awareness does not have a significant effect on<br />

perceived br<strong>and</strong>-product fit (B = -.02, t = -.72, p = .466). Thus, after controlling for br<strong>and</strong> awareness, br<strong>and</strong><br />

prototypicality has a strong <strong>and</strong> significant positive impact on perceived br<strong>and</strong>-product fit.<br />

Prototypicality has been conceptualized as the opposite of novelty or distinctiveness (Schifferstein <strong>and</strong> Hekkert, 2008;<br />

Veryzer <strong>and</strong> Hutchinson, 1998), suggesting a possible perceived fit conflict when prototypical br<strong>and</strong>s are extended to<br />

innovative products. Interestingly, the results of Study 1 suggest no such conflict. Even stronger, we find that br<strong>and</strong><br />

prototypicality increases fit perceptions between a br<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> a close novel extension. Taking a br<strong>and</strong> extension research<br />

perspective one could argue that when novel extensions are ‗close‘, consumers may perceive a level of category<br />

coherence despite the extension‘s novelty (Bridges, Keller <strong>and</strong> Sood, 2000). Because perceived fit is an important driver<br />

of innovation acceptance (e.g., Aaker <strong>and</strong> Keller, 1990; Loken <strong>and</strong> John, 1993), prototypicality probably also facilitates<br />

consumer acceptance of close novel innovations. Insights from product innovation research, however, may offer an<br />

alternative explanation. While br<strong>and</strong> extension research focuses on the effect of perceived br<strong>and</strong>-product fit to explain<br />

consumer evaluations <strong>and</strong> acceptance of extensions (e.g. Boush <strong>and</strong> Loken 1991; Keller <strong>and</strong> Aaker, 1992), product<br />

innovation research stresses the negative relation between consumers‘ perceived uncertainty about new products <strong>and</strong><br />

product acceptance (Ram <strong>and</strong> Sheth, 1989; Sorescu, Ch<strong>and</strong>y, <strong>and</strong> Prabhu, 2003). In contrast to existing products, novel<br />

products are perceived as risky, carrying uncertain outcomes (Ram <strong>and</strong> Sheth, 1989; Robertson, 1971; Golder <strong>and</strong><br />

Tellis, 1997). This uncertainty originates from perceived performance, physical, social, psychological, or financial risks<br />

(Ram <strong>and</strong> Sheth, 1989). To overcome uncertainty, consumers engage in search behavior (Urbany, Dickson <strong>and</strong> Wilkie,<br />

92

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!