08.06.2015 Views

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

Abstracts - Association for Chemoreception Sciences

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

P O S T E R S<br />

#P244 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

CENTRAL OLFACTION; CHEMOSENSORY<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS & CLINICAL STUDIES<br />

Evaluation of Newborns’ Movement by Image Segmentation<br />

while They Drink an Infant Formula<br />

Rosa G. Herrera-Lee 1, 2 , Homero V. Rios-Figueroa 1 , Jesus O.<br />

Angulo-Guerrero 2 , Julie A. Mennella 3 , Iñigo Verdalet-Guzman 1 ,<br />

Takuo Nakano 4 , Lech Ozimek 4 , Eryck R. Silva-Hernandez 1<br />

1<br />

University of Veracruz Xalapa, Mexico, 2 Veracruz Institute of<br />

Technology Veracruz, Mexico, 3 Monell Chemical Senses Center<br />

Philadelphia, PA, USA, 4 University of Alberta Edmonton, AB,<br />

Canada<br />

Babies show their com<strong>for</strong>t or discom<strong>for</strong>t through facial<br />

expressions, vocalizations and body movements. The objective of<br />

the present study was to evaluate newborns’ movement while<br />

they drink an infant <strong>for</strong>mula using a technique called image<br />

segmentation. Forty seven newborns were recruited in a local<br />

hospital in Xalapa, Mexico. The study procedures were approved<br />

by the Ethics Committee of the University of Veracruz, and<br />

in<strong>for</strong>med consent was obtained from each mother be<strong>for</strong>e the start<br />

of the study. All available milk-based and soy based infant<br />

<strong>for</strong>mulas <strong>for</strong> 0 to 6 months old babies were selected. Nine<br />

different brands of infant <strong>for</strong>mulas were included in the study.<br />

Each baby was randomly given two kinds of baby <strong>for</strong>mula. All<br />

sessions were video filmed. Each of the 94 videos were adjusted to<br />

1 min long, and then fragmented into two frames per second.<br />

A sequence program was developed in the Matlab R2008a<br />

software. The program sets a background that stays constant<br />

during the video sequence. The moving object, the newborn in<br />

this case, is analyzed. The software identifies the skin color and<br />

draws a digitalized figure of the newborn body. The resulting<br />

image is processed pixel-based using differences in color between<br />

skin and background. Two fundamental variables were measured,<br />

the bounding box area and the movement speed. It was<br />

determined that newborns moved faster when drinking soy-based<br />

<strong>for</strong>mulas, especially during the first 20 seconds of the video.<br />

In addition, faster moving infants drank less volume of infant<br />

<strong>for</strong>mulas. This technique might be useful to identify emotions in<br />

very young individuals, and to be applied in other areas such as<br />

the identification of food preferences. Acknowledgements:<br />

Thanks to CONACYT, PROMEP and the Government of<br />

Veracruz State from Mexico <strong>for</strong> their support.<br />

#P245 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

CENTRAL OLFACTION; CHEMOSENSORY<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS & CLINICAL STUDIES<br />

Factors Influencing Mothers’ Perceptions of their Infants’<br />

Liking of a Green Vegetable<br />

Catherine A. Forestell 1,2 , Julie A. Mennella 1<br />

1<br />

Monell Chemical Senses Center Philadelphia, PA, USA,<br />

2<br />

The College of William & Mary Williamsburg, VA, USA<br />

Mothers’ perceptions of their infants’ liking of foods play an<br />

important role in determining the types and variety of foods they<br />

feed to their infant. These perceptions may be influenced not only<br />

by the infants’ responses to foods, but also by various<br />

characteristics of both mother and child. The goal of the present<br />

study was to determine which factors impact maternal ratings of<br />

infants’ acceptance of a green vegetable at the time of weaning<br />

(5-10 months). To this end, 92 mother-infant dyads were tested<br />

under naturalistic conditions in which the infants determined the<br />

pace and duration of the feeding. Acceptance and liking of pureed<br />

green beans were assessed using a variety of measures, including<br />

amount and rate of consumption, and the frequency of facial<br />

distaste expressions using the anatomically-based Facial Action<br />

Coding system. At the end of each feed, mothers rated their<br />

infants’ enjoyment of the food. Although maternal ratings were<br />

not affected by socioeconomic status (e.g., mothers’ income,<br />

education level), maternal BMI, or infant temperament, ratings<br />

were lower <strong>for</strong> infants who rejected the food during the first five<br />

spoon offers, showed more facial expressions of distaste, and ate<br />

less food <strong>for</strong> a shorter period of time. When these factors were<br />

entered into a stepwise multiple regression analyses, we found<br />

that mothers’ ratings were primarily influenced by how long<br />

their infants ate, and by the number of lip raises they made<br />

during the feeding session. A better understanding of mothers’<br />

interpretations will aid in the establishment of evidence-based<br />

guidelines <strong>for</strong> feeding vegetables during infancy.<br />

Acknowledgements: The project described was supported by<br />

Award Number R01HD37119 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver<br />

National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.<br />

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does<br />

not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy<br />

Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human<br />

Development or the National Institutes of Health.<br />

#P246 POSTER SESSION V:<br />

CENTRAL OLFACTION; CHEMOSENSORY<br />

PSYCHOPHYSICS & CLINICAL STUDIES<br />

A study examining the incidence of taste disorders in the<br />

general population<br />

Antje Welge-Luessen 1 , Patrick Doerig 1 , Franziska Krone 2 ,<br />

Markus Wolfensberger 1<br />

1<br />

Dept. of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospital Basel Basel,<br />

Switzerland, 2 Smell & Taste Clinic, University of Dresden Medical<br />

School Dresden, Germany<br />

Objective: In contrast to olfaction so far little is known about the<br />

incidence of taste disorders in the general population. It was the<br />

aim of our study to examine the incidence of taste disorders using<br />

two different whole mouth tests.<br />

Subjects and Methods: Altogether 761 subjects (297 male, 464<br />

female, mean age: 36 years, range: 5 -89 years) took part in the<br />

study. Whole mouth taste function was tested using both, taste<br />

strips (dried filter papers, applied on tongue, four different<br />

concentrations of sucrose, citric acid, sodium chloride and quinine<br />

hydrochloride) and taste sprays (same substances, but higher<br />

(above threshold) concentrations). Moreover, all subjects filled in<br />

a questionnaire concerning their general health and evaluated their<br />

subjective taste ability on a visual analogue scale.Results: Taste<br />

sprays: 95% of all subjects correctly identified at least 3 out of 4<br />

sprays, 7 subjects identified only 1 out of 4 sprays, 2 subjects<br />

(1.2%) none. Taste strips: 95% identified more than 8 strips<br />

correct, 5.3% scored below 8 and 9 subjects (1.2%) scored below<br />

6. All of these were male, 2 in the age of 10 and 12 years, 4 older<br />

than 50 years. Subjectively they all rated their taste function as<br />

normal. While the two young boys also were unable to identify<br />

the sprays correctly, only one of the older males had poor results<br />

in both, sprays and taste strips. Subjective evaluation and<br />

measured function did not correlate; moreover, regarding sex and<br />

age women scored better than men and younger subjects better<br />

than older ones. Conclusion: Taste disorders are rare. Complete<br />

ageusia was not found in the examined population. Similar to the<br />

situation in olfactory function subjects are unreliable to correctly<br />

rate their taste function.<br />

108 | AChemS <strong>Abstracts</strong> 2010 <strong>Abstracts</strong> are printed as submitted by the author(s)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!